A self-determination theory approach to predicting school achievement over time: the unique role of intrinsic motivation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.08.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We test longitudinal relations of different motivation types to school achievement.

  • Intrinsic motivation is the only consistent predictor of academic achievement across different school contexts and different cultures.

  • We also find reciprocal relations between achievement and motivation types.

Abstract

Although many studies have examined the relation of academic motivation to school achievement using the Self-Determination Theory perspective, the results have been inconsistent. The present investigation represents the first systematic attempt to use a meta-analysis and controlled, longitudinal studies to examine the relations of specific types of motivation to overall academic achievement. The meta-analysis (Study 1) pointed toward a potentially important role of intrinsic motivation in predicting school achievement. Three empirical studies of high school and college students in Canada (Studies 2 and 3) and in Sweden (Study 4) showed that intrinsic motivation was the only motivation type to be consistently positively associated with academic achievement over a one-year period, controlling for baseline achievement. Amotivation was significantly associated with lower academic achievement in Studies 3 and 4. Interestingly, intrinsic motivation was also associated with reduced amotivation in two of our studies and it was reciprocally associated with higher school achievement in another study. Overall, our findings highlight the unique importance of intrinsic motivation for the future academic success of high school and college students.

Introduction

Teachers and parents all know that school motivation is crucial for academic success, which has been long known as a determinant for a host of adaptive outcomes such as school completion, career success, mental and physical health (Archambault et al, 2009, Guay et al, 2008). However, there is little agreement regarding which one should be promoted. While some researchers focus on intrinsic motivation as the most important (Deci & Ryan, 2000), others emphasize either extrinsic motivation (Wigfield & Eccles, 2000), or a combination of both intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (Elliot, Moller, 2003, Lepper et al, 2005). The present investigation examines which types of motivation are most beneficial for academic achievement, over time, in different school contexts and cultures. It also assesses whether there are reciprocal relations among academic achievement and different motivation types.

Self-Determination Theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 2000) adopts a multidimensional approach to motivation. It specifies different types of autonomous and controlled forms of intentional action. Autonomous actions are initiated by a sense of choice and personal volition, whereas controlled actions are regulated by external or internal pressures. Individuals who are controlled in their actions have an external locus of causality, whereas those who are autonomous have an internal locus of causality (DeCharms, 1968). Intrinsic motivation is viewed as the prototype of autonomy (Deci, Ryan, 1985, Deci, Ryan, 2000, Lepper et al, 1973). When intrinsically motivated, individuals freely engage in an interesting activity simply for the enjoyment and excitement it brings, rather than to get a reward or to satisfy a constraint (Deci & Ryan, 1985). They perceive themselves as the causal agent of their own behaviour (DeCharms, 1968). By contrast, extrinsic motivation is instrumental in nature. Behaviour that is extrinsically motivated is not performed out of interest, but for the consequence it is thought to be instrumentally linked to (Wrzesniewski et al., 2014). Extrinsic motivation is thought to be important for socially prescribed activities, such as doing homework, because they are often not inherently interesting. Unlike many conceptualizations of motivation (e.g., Harter, 1981), SDT does not view extrinsic motivation as one-dimensional and opposed to intrinsic motivation. Instead, it specifies different types of extrinsic motivation, which vary in the extent to which they are autonomous (Deci & Ryan, 1985).

These types, from the least to the most autonomous, consist of external, introjected and identified regulation. External regulation refers to behaviours that are initiated by an external contingency, for example, being offered a reward to do one's homework. Introjected regulation refers to internalizing a regulation without fully accepting it into one's sense of self. It involves feelings of internal coercion and pressure, and refers to attempts to avoid feeling unworthy, guilty or ashamed, or to prove one's worth (Assor, Vansteenkiste, & Kaplan, 2009). An example of introjected regulation would be a student who studies long hours to prove to herself that she is worthy. Identified regulation takes place when the value of an instrumental behaviour has come to be identified with one's sense of self. This type of regulation is considered to be more autonomous than the other types of extrinsic motivation because it is initiated from a sense of personal meaning and volition (Deci, Ryan, 2000, Koestner, Losier, 2002). A student who does extra exercises at the end of a history chapter because she believes it will help her fully understand the subject matter is regulated by identification.1 SDT also considers amotivation, the absence of motivation that happens when an individual does not experience intentionality or a sense of personal causation. These different forms of motivation have been proposed to lie along a continuum of relative autonomy, starting with the form that exhibits the lowest level to the one that represents the highest level of autonomy (Deci & Ryan, 1985). SDT (Ryan & Connell, 1989) also mentions that, adjacent motivations on the continuum (e.g., intrinsic motivation and identified regulation) should relate more strongly to each other than distal ones (e.g., intrinsic motivation and external regulation). However, evidence for the continuum is inconsistent. While some findings corroborate this pattern, others deviate from it in various ways (e.g., intrinsic motivation being more strongly related to introjected than to identified regulation) (for examples, see Boiché et al, 2008, Ntoumanis, 2002, Otis et al, 2005, Ratelle et al, 2007).

Although many studies have examined the relation of academic motivation to school achievement from the SDT perspective (e.g., Chatzisarantis et al, 2003, Deci et al, 1991), the majority have been cross-sectional and have yielded inconsistent results (Cokley et al, 2001, D'Ailly, 2003, Fortier et al, 1995, Grolnick et al, 1991, Hardre, Reeve, 2003, Noels et al, 1999, Otis et al, 2005, Petersen et al, 2009, Soenens, Vansteenkiste, 2005, Walls, Little, 2005). A careful examination of past research shows that only a few studies have adopted a prospective design while also controlling for previous achievement (Baker, 2003, Black, Deci, 2000, Burton et al, 2006, Study 2b). The details of these controlled prospective studies merit review. Burton et al. (2006) conducted a 6-week prospective study of university students to examine the relations of intrinsic motivation and identified regulation to final exam performance in a single psychology course. Results showed that, controlling for previous grades, identified regulation significantly positively predicted final examination grades whereas intrinsic motivation was unrelated to the final grades. Black and Deci (2000) examined the relation of relative autonomy in a sample of college chemistry students over a one-semester period. They found that relative autonomy did not significantly predict final course grade, after controlling for previous ability and grade point average (GPA). Results for specific types of motivation were not reported. Finally, Baker (2003) examined the relations of academic motivation types to total GPA in a sample of university students and controlled for academic achievement as measured by entry qualifications upon entering university. Her results showed that intrinsic motivation, assessed during the second semester of the first year of university, was the only type of motivation to significantly predict overall academic performance measured one year later, controlling for entry qualifications.

An example of another longitudinal study that has controlled for baseline achievement is one by (Guay, Ratelle, Roy, & Litalien, 2010). Using a cross-lagged model to examine the reciprocal relations of academic motivation and achievement in a population of high school students, they found that autonomous motivation, as defined by a relative autonomy score, was positively associated with academic achievement over the course of one year, even after controlling for baseline achievement. However, they did not estimate the contribution of each type of motivation to later achievement, making it difficult to understand which type of motivation was driving this relation.

Given the inconsistent results of past cross-sectional studies and the paucity of longitudinal studies that have controlled for baseline levels of achievement, a more systematic review of the research is needed in order to fully understand the effect of each different motivation type on school achievement. Moreover, as Ratelle et al. (2007) have suggested, more longitudinal studies are necessary to provide some information about the causal mechanisms between motivation and achievement. Finally, no studies have examined these longitudinal relations in samples of high school students. Since failure to achieve is a prevalent problem in high school and leads to undesirable consequences such as dropout, it is imperative to conduct carefully controlled studies in such a pre-university population.

In an attempt to overcome the limitations of past research, we conducted a meta-analysis and a series of three empirical studies to systematically examine the contribution of the different motivation types to school achievement. The meta-analysis (Study 1) reviewed cross-sectional and longitudinal studies that have assessed the relation of motivation types to school achievement according to SDT, using the Academic Motivation Scale (AMS) designed by Vallerand et al. (1992). This is the most widely used scale of school motivation from the SDT framework. We also designed three controlled, longitudinal studies that used the AMS to measure five different types of academic motivation and to examine their relation to school achievement over time. To ensure that our findings were robust and generalizable, we varied the school context and cultural context across the three studies. Studies 2 and 4 included high school students, while Study 3 included college students. Studies 2 and 3 included Canadian students, while Study 4 included students from Sweden. In each study, we controlled for baseline levels of achievement. SDT also predicts that different motivation types relate to each other but past research examining these relations longitudinally is scarce and does not provide a clear picture (Guay et al, 2010, Otis et al, 2005). We thus tested the relations among different types of motivation in an exploratory way. Finally, we examined the reciprocal relations between academic motivation and achievement since some studies that have tested cross-lagged models have shown that prior academic achievement predict subsequent academic motivation (Garon-Carrier et al, 2014, Goldberg, Cornell, 1998).

Study 2 examined the relations of motivation types to school achievement over a one-year period in a large Canadian high school sample. Study 3 examined the relations of different types of motivation to achievement after the transition from high school to college, which is compulsory after high school in the Canadian province of Québec for those wanting to pursue university studies. Study 4 examined the relations of motivation types to school achievement in a sample of high school students attending their final year of the science stream in Sweden.

Section snippets

Study 1

In this meta-analysis, we compiled results of cross-sectional and prospective studies employing the Academic Motivation Scale (AMS; Vallerand et al., 1992). An investigation of each motivation subtype was conducted to assess its relations to academic achievement. Our hypotheses were as follows. First, in line with SDT, we predicted that intrinsic and identified regulation would each have a positive association with school achievement, and thus a positive effect size. Second, we expected a

Study 2

The meta-analysis presented above highlighted that a large proportion of past studies comparing the effects of different motivation types have been cross-sectional or have assessed motivation and achievement over time without taking baseline levels into account. Furthermore, all of these controlled prospective studies were conducted solely with college or university students, and two of them included relatively short-term follow-ups or did not report results for all individual types of

Study 3

We conducted Study 3 to test the whether the findings obtained in Study 2 could extend to a population of college students enrolled in a science program. One advantage of this study was that objective grades were obtained as a measure of academic achievement. Another advantage was the focus on students in science, an area that is known for higher base rates of dropout than other fields of study (Lavigne, Vallerand, & Miquelon, 2007). In fact, almost 30% of Canadian and American college students

Study 4

Study 4 aimed to replicate the relation of different academic motivation types to academic motivation found in Studies 2 and 3 and to extend these results cross-culturally. In this study, we explored the relation between these variables in a sample of Swedish high school students who had chosen to study natural sciences. Because SDT postulates that autonomy is a basic psychological need that is relevant to all humans, it becomes important to examine the relation of different types of motivation

General discussion

The objective of the present investigation was to examine the relations of different types of motivation to overall academic achievement in order to adequately test which types play the most important role. We first performed a meta-analysis of the literature showing that, in general, intrinsic motivation and identified regulation (i.e., both types of autonomous motivation) have the strongest positive relations to academic achievement, whereas amotivation has the strongest negative relation.

Conclusion

Many studies have shown that autonomous motivation is positively related to academic achievement (Niemiec & Ryan, 2009). However, these relations have rarely been investigated in prospective studies of high school students that controlled for baseline achievement. The objective of the present investigation was to use a controlled prospective design to examine whether these motivation types could predict changes in academic achievement over time, during the transition from high school to

References (91)

  • ArchambaultI. et al.

    Adolescent behavioral, affective, and cognitive engagement in school: Relationship to dropout

    The Journal of School Health

    (2009)
  • AssorA. et al.

    Identified versus introjected approach and introjected avoidance motivations in school and in sports: The limited benefits of self worth strivings

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2009)
  • BenwareC.A. et al.

    Quality of learning with an active versus passive motivational set

    American Educational Research Journal

    (1984)
  • BlackA.E. et al.

    The effects of instructors’ autonomy support and students’ autonomous motivation on learning organic chemistry: A self-determination theory perspective

    Science Education

    (2000)
  • BoichéJ.C.S. et al.

    Students’ motivational profiles and achievement outcomes in physical education: A self-determination perspective

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2008)
  • BuhiE.R. et al.

    Out of sight, not out of mind: Strategies for handling missing data

    American Journal of Health Behavior

    (2008)
  • BurtonK.D. et al.

    The differential effects of intrinsic and identified motivation on well-being and performance: Prospective, experimental, and implicit approaches to self-determination theory

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2006)
  • ChatzisarantisN.L.D. et al.

    A meta-analysis of perceived locus of causality in exercise, sport, and physical education contexts

    Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology

    (2003)
  • ChirkovV. et al.

    Parent and teacher autonomy-support in Russian and U.S. adolescents: Common effects on well-being and academic motivation

    Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology

    (2001)
  • CohenJ.

    A power primer

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1992)
  • CokleyK.O. et al.

    A psychometric investigation of the Academic Motivation Scale using a United States sample

    Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development

    (2001)
  • D'AillyH.

    Children's autonomy and perceived control in learning: A model of motivation and achievement in Taiwan

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2003)
  • DeciE.L. et al.

    Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior

    (1985)
  • DeciE.L. et al.

    The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior

    Psychological Inquiry

    (2000)
  • DeciE.L. et al.

    A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1999)
  • DeciE.L. et al.

    Overview of Self-Determination Theory: An organismic dialectical perspective

  • DeciE.L. et al.

    Motivation and education: The self-determination perspective

    Educational Psychologist

    (1991)
  • DeCharmsR.

    Personal causation: The internal affective determinants of behavior

    (1968)
  • DornbuschS.M. et al.

    The relation of parenting style to adolescent school performance

    Child Development

    (1987)
  • DuchesneS. et al.

    Adjustment trajectories in college science programs: Perceptions of qualities of parents’ and college teachers’ relationships

    Journal of Counseling Psychology

    (2007)
  • EcclesJ.S. et al.

    Motivational beliefs, values, and goal

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2002)
  • EisingaR. et al.

    The reliability of a two-item scale: Pearson, Cronbach, or Spearman-Brown?

    International Journal of Public Health

    (2013)
  • FieldA.P. et al.

    How to do a meta-analysis

    British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology

    (2010)
  • GagnéM. et al.

    Self-determination theory and work motivation

    Journal of Organizational Behavior

    (2005)
  • Garon-Carrier, G., Boivin, M., Forget-Dubois, N., Guay, F., Kovas, Y., Lemelin, J.-P., et al. (2014). Une étude...
  • GoldbergM.D. et al.

    The influence of intrinsic motivation and self concept on academic achievement in second- and third-grade students

    Journal for the Education of the Gifted

    (1998)
  • GrolnickW.S. et al.

    What makes parents controlling?

  • GrolnickW.S. et al.

    Inner resources for school achievement: Motivational mediators of children's perceptions of their parents

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1991)
  • GuayF. et al.

    Optimal learning in optimal contexts: The role of self-determination in education

    Canadian Psychology

    (2008)
  • HardreP.L. et al.

    A motivational model of rural students’ intentions to persist in, versus drop out of, high school

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2003)
  • HarterS.

    A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the classroom: Motivational and informational components

    Developmental Psychology

    (1981)
  • HedgesL.V. et al.

    Statistical methods for meta-analysis

    (1985)
  • HennanM.R. et al.

    The influence of family regulation, connection, and psychological autonomy on six measures of adolescent functioning

    Journal of Adolescent Research

    (1997)
  • HofstedeG.

    National cultures in four dimensions: A research-based theory of cultural differences among nations

    International Studies of Management & Organization

    (1983)
  • JangH. et al.

    Can self-determination theory explain what underlies the productive, satisfying learning experiences of collectivistically oriented Korean students?

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2009)
  • Cited by (416)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Present address: Lund University, Department of Psychology, SE-221 00 Lund, Sweden.

    View full text