Is effort praise motivational? The role of beliefs in the effort–ability relationship

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2008.01.005Get rights and content

Abstract

In two studies, we investigated how beliefs in the effort–ability relationship moderated the effects of effort praise on student motivation. Study 1 showed that the more the participants believed that effort and ability were related positively (the positive rule) versus related negatively (the inverse rule), the more they would have positive self-evaluation and intrinsic motivation after effort praise. Study 2, with participants’ beliefs manipulated by a priming procedure, showed that the participants in the positive rule condition had better self-evaluation and more intrinsic motivation after effort praise than their counterparts did in the inverse rule condition. The results of the two studies converged to indicate that the motivational effects of effort praise depend on beliefs in the effort–ability relationship.

Introduction

Praise is commonly used to motivate children in learning (Brophy, 1981, Emmer, 1987, Hitz and Driscoll, 1988). Among various types of praise, effort praise has been found to best foster adaptive achievement responses. Children receiving effort praise were found to show higher levels of interest and challenge seeking than children receiving other forms of praise. For example, in a series of studies, Mueller and Dweck (1998) found that praise for effort (i.e., “You must have worked hard at these problems.”) had more positive consequences for children’s task enjoyment, persistence, and performance than praise for intelligence (i.e., “You must be smart at these problems.”). Similarly, Kamins and Dweck (1999) found that, in the face of setbacks, children displayed less helpless responses on self-assessment, affects, and persistence after effort praise than ability praise. The positive effects of effort praise are most likely due to the mechanism of effort attribution (Weiner, 1985). Effort praise encourages children to attribute their learning outcomes to effort, an internal and controllable factor. This conveys a message to children that they can remedy a failure situation and are in control of their learning outcomes.

Despite the evidence for the positive effects of effort praise, there are also studies showing otherwise. Miller, Brickman, and Bolen (1975) found that children who were praised for working hard did not improve their performances as much as children who were told that they had “excellent ability”. Similarly, Schunk, 1982, Schunk, 1983 also found that children who were praised for their efforts showed less skill acquisition and self-efficacy than children praised for their abilities. These results were replicated in samples of college students (Koestner, Zuckerman, & Koestner, 1987) and Chinese children (Hau & Salili, 1996). Schunk (1983) explains the superiority of ability praise over effort praise on the basis of self-efficacy theory (Bandura, 1977). He reasons that ability praise should produce higher expectations for future performance than effort praise because of the stronger competence information.

In view of these inconsistent findings, the positive effects of effort praise are equivocal. Is effort praise motivational or not? This is a bewildering question that has significant implications for educational practices. A constructive approach to this question is to sort out the conditions in which effort praise has or does not have positive effects. In the past, researchers have identified two conditions that account for inconsistent findings about the effects of effort praise; namely the type of involvement and the presence of subsequent setbacks.

Section snippets

Involvement and setbacks

In an experiment with college students, Koestner and his colleageus (1987) found an interesting interaction between type of involvement and praise. They found that the students who received effort praise were relatively more intrinsically motivated under task-involving than ego-involving situations. However, they also found that the students who received ability praise were relatively more motivated under ego-involving than task-involving situations. According to Nicholls (1984), the goal in a

Beliefs in the effort and ability relationship

Children may espouse two different beliefs in the relationship between effort and ability. They may believe that effort and ability are related inversely (the inverse rule), that is, the less one’s ability, the more one has to make an effort for success. On the other hand, they may believe that effort and ability are related positively (the positive rule), that is, the more one exerts effort, the higher is one’s ability. We speculate that if children believe the inverse rule, they will be

Overview of the present research

We argue that beliefs in the effort–ability relationship may be subject to individual differences. These individual differences can moderate the effects of effort praise on motivation. When students adopt a static perspective and focus on the inverse relationship between effort and ability, they will be discouraged when they receive effort praise because high effort implies low ability. In contrast, when students adopt a dynamic perspective and focus on the positive relationship between effort

Participants

The participants were 34 7th graders from a Hong Kong secondary school in a middle-lower class neighborhood. They participated in the study voluntarily with parental consent. The data of six participants were excluded as they had poor performances in the experimental task and therefore were not praised. As a result, the final data set consisted of 28 students (15 boys and 13 girls).

Procedures

The study was conducted in a group setting after school on a normal school day. The students gathered in their

Participants

The participants were 45 7th graders from a Hong Kong secondary school in a middle-lower class neighborhood. With the help of the school personnel, invitation letters were sent home to all the 7th graders (N = 150). Parents were informed that we invited their children to participate in our study that would be conducted during the extra-curricular activities period at the end of the school year. They were also informed that participants could attend a free educational workshop on learning

General discussion

Effort praise has been used extensively by adults to influence children’s behavior. It is important for researchers and educators to understand how it affects children’s motivation. Past research has shown inconsistent findings about the effects of effort praise on children’s motivation. We agree with Henderlong and Lepper (2002) that praise is a complex social phenomenon and its effects are subject to many conditions. Therefore, it is more useful to ask about the conditions under which effort

Conclusion

The present research reveals the importance of beliefs in the effort–ability relationship. It shows that these beliefs moderate the effects of effort praise on children’s motivation. Effort praise can be motivational when the recipient believes in a positive relationship between effort and ability. However, it can be de-motivational when the recipient believes in an inverse relationship. The present research has made a unique contribution to the existing body of knowledge by identifying a

References (36)

  • B. Kurtz-Costes et al.

    What does it mean to be smart? The development of children’s beliefs about intelligence in Germany and the United States

    Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology

    (2005)
  • A. Bandura

    Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change

    Psychological Review

    (1977)
  • A. Bandura et al.

    Self-evaluative and self-efficacy mechanisms governing the motivational effects of goal systems

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1983)
  • G.P. Barker et al.

    Developmental study of praise and blame as attributional cues

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1987)
  • J. Brophy

    Teacher praise: A functional analysis

    Review of Educational Research

    (1981)
  • E. Deci et al.

    Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior

    (1985)
  • C.S. Dweck

    Motivational processes affecting learning

    American Psychologist

    (1986)
  • Education Commission (2000). Learning for life, learning through life: Reform proposal for the education system in Hong...
  • E.T. Emmer

    Praise and the instructional process

    Journal of Classroom Interaction

    (1987)
  • K.T. Hau et al.

    Motivational effects of teachers’ ability versus effort feedback on Chinese students’ learning

    Social Psychology of Education

    (1996)
  • F. Heidier

    The psychology of interpersonal relations

    (1958)
  • J. Henderlong et al.

    The effects of praise on children’s intrinsic motivation: A review and synthesis

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2002)
  • V.L. Henderson et al.

    Achievement and motivation

  • R. Hitz et al.

    Praise or encouragement? New insights into praise: Implication for early childhood teachers

    Young Children

    (1988)
  • Y.-y. Hong

    Chinese students’ and teachers’ inferences of effort and ability

  • Y.-y. Hong et al.

    Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1999)
  • M.L. Kamins et al.

    Person versus process praise and criticism: Implications for contingent self-worth and coping

    Developmental Psychology

    (1999)
  • D.E. Kanouse et al.

    The semantics of praise

  • Cited by (0)

    We wish to thank Edna Chan, Rebecca Cheng, Lisa Ho and Woody Lee for their assistance in data collection. We are also grateful for the support of our participants and the assistance provided by their schools.

    View full text