Elsevier

Behaviour Research and Therapy

Volume 100, January 2018, Pages 67-77
Behaviour Research and Therapy

A randomised controlled trial investigating the benefits of adaptive working memory training for working memory capacity and attentional control in high worriers

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2017.10.011Get rights and content

Highlights

  • n-back training does not significantly improve working memory or worry beyond the effects of 1-back training.

  • Improvements on the n-back task were associated with gains in working memory capacity and reductions in worry symptoms.

  • Individual differences may be a relevant factor in the efficacy of training.

Abstract

The process of worry has been associated with reductions in working memory capacity and availability of resources necessary for efficient attentional control. This, in turn, can lead to escalating worry. Recent investigations into working memory training have shown improvements in attentional control and cognitive performance in high trait-anxious individuals and individuals with sub-clinical depression. The current randomised controlled trial investigated the effects of 15 days of adaptive n-back working memory training, or an active control task, on working memory capacity, attentional control and worry in a sample of high worriers. Pre-training, post-training and one-month follow-up measures of working memory capacity were assessed using a Change Detection task, while a Flanker task was used to assess attentional control. A breathing focus task was used as a behavioural measure of worry in addition to a number of self-report assessments of worry and anxiety. Overall there was no difference between the active training and the active control condition with both groups demonstrating similar improvements in working memory capacity and worry, post-training and at follow-up. However, training-related improvements on the n-back task were associated with gains in working memory capacity and reductions in worry symptoms in the active training condition. These results highlight the need for further research investigating the role of individual differences in working memory training.

Introduction

Worry is defined as “the repeated experience of apparently uncontrollable thoughts regarding future negative events” (Hayes, Hirsch, & Mathews, 2008, p. 3). It occurs on a continuum, with generalised anxiety disorder (GAD) falling at the more severe end (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). GAD affects an estimated 4.4% of people in England (McManus, Meltzer, Brugha, Bebbington, & Jenkins, 2009), while 6.2% of the adult population of the United States of America will also experience GAD during their lifetime (Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky, & Wittchen, 2012). This results in severe disability or limitations to usual activities for 42% of sufferers (Sanderson & Andrews, 2002). Within the United Kingdom, treatment guidelines recommend Cognitive Behavioural Therapy or selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors for individuals with marked functional impairment as a result of significant worry (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2011).

Worry can have a disruptive influence on major cognitive functions, including working memory, which play a crucial role in every day functioning. It has been suggested that worry acts as an internal distracter, attracting attention away from a current task, and thus reducing the capacity for attentional control (Hirsch & Mathews, 2012). Working memory capacity (WMC), a concept that is closely related to attentional control (Shipstead, Lindsey, Marshall, & Engle, 2014), reflects “the efficacy by which goal relevant information is attended, stored, and maintained while task irrelevant information is suppressed” (Sari, Koster, & Derakshan, 2017, p. 2.). When WMC is reduced, attentional control is also affected, since an individual has fewer resources accessible for voluntary, top-down control of attention towards current task demands (Hirsch & Mathews, 2012). For instance, trait vulnerability to worry (Owens, Derakshan, & Richards, 2015) as well as an episode of active worrying (Sari et al., 2017) can reduce WMC, affecting major executive functions such as inhibition of irrelevant material, leading to the inefficient processing of relevant information.

In support of this perspective, worry-prone individuals have been shown to perform less well on tasks requiring attentional control. Stout, Shackman, Johnson, and Larson (2015), for instance, found that individual differences in subjective worry are associated with difficulties gating threat distracters from working memory during a change detection task. Moreover, once threat-related information is in working memory, it biases attention, even if the threat is no longer present. This mechanism produces a vicious cycle leading to escalating levels of worry. Further evidence comes from the observation that individuals with (pre-existing) limited attentional control resources, as reported on a self-report scale, are particularly vulnerable to bias towards threat, as indicated by difficulties disengaging from threatening information during a simple detection task (Derryberry & Reed, 2002). Moreover, Stefanopoulou, Hirsch, Hayes, Adlam, and Coker (2014) invited participants to complete a random key-pressing task while thinking about a worrisome event and observed that worry consumed more attentional control resources in individuals with GAD than in control participants, as indicated by a decrease in the proportion of random key presses by participants with GAD (but not controls) when thinking about a worrying event, compared to a positive event. Stefanopoulou et al. also demonstrated that people with GAD had greater difficulty than controls in sustaining attention when demands on attentional control were increased during a n-back task. These results largely replicated those found by Hayes et al. (2008) who had also used a random key-press task to demonstrate that high worriers show more restricted WMC, and hence fewer attentional resources, when thinking about a current worry than when thinking about a positive topic, an effect not found in low worriers. Similarly, Sari et al. (2017) found that compared to a non-worry control condition, students undergoing a period of active worry demonstrated impaired WMC, and this effect was mediated by self-reported worry.

A recent meta-analysis (Moran, 2016) has confirmed the consistent link between anxiety or worry and limitations in WMC, and how the combination of high worry and low WMC, appears to reduce the cognitive resources available for attentional control. This body of work leads to the suggestion that it may be valuable to train WMC in high worriers. By training WMC, the efficiency of attentional control is likely to increase, which should in turn reduce worry, as there will be more mental resources available to exert cognitive control over whether negative thoughts attract attention.

One issue associated with many of the studies discussed above is the subjective nature of the measurements used to assess worry. Such methods are often susceptible to bias themselves and therefore it is useful to also obtain a more objective measure of worry. A widely-used method was introduced by Borkovec, Robinson, Pruzinsky, and DePree (1983) that involves an assessment of participants' ability to focus on their breathing before and after a time-period of worrying. An adaptation of this task was used by Fox, Dutton, Yates, Georgiou, and Mouchlianitis (2015) to assess the effectiveness of a cognitive training procedure designed to improve attentional control on the reduction of worry. Although the cognitive training procedure (based on the flanker task) was largely ineffective, they found that improvements in attentional control were associated with participants’ ability to suppress worry-related thought intrusions during a breathing-focus task similar to that used by Borkevec et al. Such findings highlight the value of including both subjective and objective measures of worry during the assessment of interventions targeting a reduction in worry.

A growing body of research is investigating whether it is possible to train WMC. For example, Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Jonides, and Perrig (2008) successfully used an adaptive dual n-back task to train the WMC of healthy participants. In the dual n-back task, participants are required to make a decision about whether paired visual and auditory stimuli match those presented n trials back, with difficulty increasing up to the 4-back level. Task difficulty is adjusted relative to task performance, thus providing adaptive training, and resulting in load on working memory gradually increasing over time. Jaeggi et al. found that, compared to a non-adaptive control condition, as few as 17 days of training using the dual n-back task led to improvements in both WMC and fluid intelligence, as measured by the Bochumer Matrizen-Test. Importantly, this test was entirely unrelated to the n-back task, indicating a transfer of training effects. Despite findings such as that by Jaeggi et al., there is still much debate about the effectiveness of working memory training. Indeed, in a meta-analytic review of 87 publications reporting on the impact of WMT on a variety of cognitive functions, Melby-Lervåg, Redick, and Hulme (2016) concluded that, while there was evidence for improvements on near transfer tasks (e.g., verbal and visuospatial working memory), there was no evidence for far-transfer to other cognitive abilities such as reading comprehension, mathematical ability, or general measures of verbal or nonverbal ability. This extensive review did not, however, consider the potential benefits of WMT in emotionally vulnerable populations.

There has been much recent debate about the potential benefits of adaptive working memory training (WMT) in emotionally vulnerable populations, such as those with depression or anxiety (see Keshavan et al., 2014, Koster et al., 2017, Motter et al., 2016 for discussion). To illustrate, Owens, Koster, and Derakshan (2013) found that, compared to a non-adaptive training control group using a 1-back version of WMT, eight days of adaptive working memory training on the dual n-back task led to training related gains in WMC, as measured by a change-detection task and these gains correlated with improvements in inhibitory function as measured by electroencephalography (EEG). In a follow-up study, high trait anxious participants were randomly assigned to 15 sessions of adaptive dual n-back or non-adaptive dual 1-back training (Sari, Koster, Pourtois, & Derakshan, 2016). The three weeks of adaptive training led to significant improvements in attentional control, as measured by a Flanker task and resting state EEG, and these gains were associated with a reduction in trait anxiety. Furthermore, Sari et al. (2016) observed that the level of improvement on the n-back task was significantly associated with reductions in anxiety symptomology. This indicates that the degree of engagement with the training task may predict the extent to which training leads to far-transfer improvements on emotional domains, such as anxiety.

Hadwin and Richards (2016) investigated the effects of WMT in children who reported elevated anxiety and reduced attentional control. Elevated anxiety was indicated by a score above the average population norm on the Spence Children's Anxiety Scale (Spence, 1998), while reduced attentional control was signified by a below median score on the attention subscale of the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire Revised (Ellis & Rothbart, 2001). Thirty-six participants aged eleven-to-fourteen years were randomly allocated to an adaptive WMT group or an active cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) control group. Children in the adaptive group completed twenty-five sessions of WMT, over a period of five weeks, while the control group involved ten bi-weekly 1-h sessions of group CBT, also over a period of five weeks. Hadwin and Richards found that both interventions were equally effective at reducing anxiety symptoms, increasing inhibitory control and reducing attentional biases to threat. These improvements were maintained at three-month follow-up. Since WMT does not require regular sessions with a therapist, it has the potential to be a cost-effective alternative, or adjunct to CBT, suggestion that WMT may be a plausible low-level intervention for protecting against the development of clinical anxiety in at risk individuals.1

Course-Choi, Saville, and Derakshan (2017) compared seven days of dual n-back training with dual n-back training in combination with mindfulness meditation practice and separate mindfulness meditation practice. A reduction in self-reported worry was observed in the n-back training-only and mindfulness-only groups, with the greatest decrease occurring in the combined group. Furthermore, WMC, as measured by a Change Detection Task, improved in all three groups as a result of training. This study highlights the potential of WMT when used in conjunction with more well-established psychological interventions. In contrast to reviews finding little evidence for “far transfer” effects of WMT in non-clinical populations (e.g. Melby-Lervåg et al., 2016), there is growing evidence that computerised cognitive training might be of benefit for clinical populations. A systematic review of computerised cognitive interventions for individuals with major depressive disorder reported moderate-to-large effect sizes for multiple cognitive domains, including attention and working memory along with small-to-moderate improvements in symptom severity (Motter et al., 2016). Similarly, a recent systematic review by Koster et al (2017) found that cognitive control training interventions for depression, such as the n-back task, may have the greatest impact for those who are most cognitively impaired and/or most engaged with the training intervention.

Given the evidence that, compared to the general population, the WMC and attentional control of high worriers are particularly impeded, it may be that WMT is of particular value for this population, as there is greater scope for gain in these domains. There is therefore an urgent need to systematically and rigorously evaluate whether or not WMT is an effective intervention for individual who are worry-prone.

Given the hypothesized role of working memory difficulties in the maintenance of worry, WMT may have some benefits for reducing worry in high-worriers. Since significant levels of worry have a negative impact on day-to-day activity involving cognitive functions (Sanderson & Andrews, 2002) even a small improvement may have a significant impact on an individual's wellbeing. The current study investigated the effects of an adaptive visual n-back WMT on the ability to control worry in a sample of high worriers. The effects of training were assessed immediately post-training and at a four-week follow-up. Training effects of WMT were assessed using a Change Detection task, which is a widely-used measure of WMC (Vogel, McCollough, & Machizawa, 2005). The effect of WMT on inhibitory control was assessed using a modified version of the Flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974), which assesses participants' ability to focus on a specific stimulus, while inhibiting attention to distractor stimuli. The effects of WMT on self-reported symptoms of trait anxiety and worry were assessed using the Spielberger Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983), the Worry Domains Questionnaire (Tallis, Eysenck, & Mathews, 1992), and the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (Meyer, Miller, Metzger, & Borkovec, 1990). In order to minimise the effects of response bias and provide a more objective, behavioural, measure of worry, this study also included a breathing focus task based on the protocol of Hirsch, Hayes, and Mathews (2009), which itself was an adaptation of the task introduced by Borkovec et al. (1983). This was a novel aspect of the current study, which effectively provides a far-transfer measure of participants' worry symptoms.

We predicted that, compared to an active control group, adaptive visual n-back WMT would improve WMC, as measured by a Change Detection task, as well as improve performance on the flanker task. We predicted that these improvements would lead to an increase in top-down attentional control which would inhibit competing threat representations and subsequently reduce worry symptomatology (Hirsch & Mathews, 2012). We also expected to replicate Sari et al.’s (2016) observation that training-related gains were associated with reduced symptomology. We therefore predicted an association between improvement on the training task and reductions in worry and anxiety on subjective as well as objective (behavioural) measures.

Section snippets

Study design

Participants were randomised to complete either 15 days of online adaptive visual n-back WMT training or 15 days of non-adaptive visual 1-back WMT. Participants completed self-report and objective measures at baseline, immediately post-training, and at a four-week follow-up. The study was reviewed and approved by the University of Oxford's Central University Research Ethics Committee (Ref: MSD-IDREC-C1-2014-215).

Participants

Participants were high worriers who scored 56 or above on the Penn State Worry

Sample characteristics

Sample characteristics for the final sample are displayed in Table 1. This includes demographic characteristics, scores on the PSWQ screening and whether the participant satisfied formal diagnostic criteria for GAD, or any other anxiety disorder (GAD, phobia, social anxiety, panic disorder, agoraphobia or hypochondriasis), as assessed by the MINI. Income or socioeconomic status was not measured. There were no significant differences between control and training groups for the number of

Discussion

The current study investigated whether adaptive visual n-back WMT can improve working memory and its associated attentional control processes in worry-prone individuals. It also aimed to assess whether any such improvements would generalise to reductions in self-reported worry and anxiety, as well as to performance on a behavioural measure of worry (a breathing focus task). A main finding of the current investigation was that adaptive n-back training led to similar improvements in working

Conclusion

Although adaptive WMT led to improvements in the WMC of worry-prone individuals, such improvements were not above-and-beyond the benefits of control training. However, of interest was the observation that training-related improvements on the n-back task were associated with reductions in self-reported worry symptomatology. It would therefore be helpful for future research to investigate the factors underlying this association.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a BA/Leverhulme Small Research Grant - 2014-2015 [SG142989] awarded to Elaine Fox and Nazanin Derakshan. Elaine Fox is supported by the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013)/ERC grant agreement no: [324176].

References (56)

  • Z. Shipstead et al.

    The mechanisms of working memory capacity: Primary memory, secondary memory, and attention control

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (2014)
  • S.H. Spence

    A measure of anxiety symptoms among children

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1998)
  • J. Stober

    Reliability and validity of two widely-used worry questionnaires: Self- report and self-peer convergence

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (1998)
  • F. Tallis et al.

    A questionnaire for the measurement of nonpathological worry

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (1992)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

    (2013)
  • J. Au et al.

    Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: A meta-analysis

    Psychonomic Bulletin Review

    (2015)
  • L.L.B. Barnes et al.

    Reliability generalization of scores on the Spielberger state-trait anxiety inventory

    Educational and Psychological Measurement

    (2002)
  • C.C. von Bastian et al.

    Effects and mechanisms of working memory training: A review

    Psychological Research

    (2014)
  • J. Course-Choi et al.

    The effects of adaptive working memory training and mindfulness meditation training on processing efficiency and worry in high worriers

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2017)
  • J.R. Crawford

    Current and premorbid intelligence measures in neuropsychological assessment

  • D. Derryberry et al.

    Anxiety-related biases and their regulation by attentional control

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2002)
  • L.K. Ellis et al.

    Revision of the early adolescent temperament questionnaire

  • B.A. Eriksen et al.

    Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a non search task

    Perception and Psychophysics

    (1974)
  • M.W. Eysenck et al.

    Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory

    Emotion

    (2007)
  • E. Fox et al.

    Attentional control and suppressing negative thought intrusions in pathological worry

    Clinical Psychological Science

    (2015)
  • R.O. Frost et al.

    The dimensions of perfectionism

    Cognitive Therapy and Research

    (1990)
  • J.A. Hadwin et al.

    Working memory training and CBT reduces anxiety symptoms and attentional biases to threat: A preliminary study

    Frontiers in Psychology

    (2016)
  • S. Hayes et al.

    Restriction of working memory capacity during worry

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2008)
  • Cited by (26)

    • EEG correlates of neutral working memory training induce attentional control improvements in test anxiety

      2022, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      This between-task difference may be responsible for the observed between-task significance difference in beneficial transfer effects on attentional control resources (due to neutral working memory training), that is a significant result for the Go/Nogo task and no significant result for the Flanker task. Previous experimental studies showed that neutral working memory training could reduce anxiety symptoms in high anxious individuals (Beloe & Derakshan, 2019; Hotton et al., 2018; Zhao et al., 2020). It is known that an attentional control deficit, which is typical for HTA individuals, may form a risk for further developing anxiety symptoms (Zainal & Newman, 2018).

    • Individual differences associated with treatment adherence and transfer effects following gamified web-based cognitive control training for repetitive negative thinking

      2022, Internet Interventions
      Citation Excerpt :

      At the same time, however, the CCT literature also contains a number of inconsistent findings (for recent reviews, see Koster et al., 2017; Van den Bergh et al., 2018). For instance, Hotton et al. (2018) failed to observe beneficial effects of CCT on RNT or anxiety in a sample of high worriers. In addition, Van den Bergh et al. (2020) observed no additive effects of CCT in terms of self-reported RNT, and level of anxiety- or depressive symptoms in the context of a cognitive behavior therapy program targeting fear of failure.

    • Rethinking cognitive training: The moderating roles of emotional vulnerability and perceived cognitive impact of training in high worriers

      2021, Behaviour Research and Therapy
      Citation Excerpt :

      The n-back training has also reduced anxious and depressive symptomatology immediately after training and at a one-month follow-up in adolescents (Beloe & Derakshan, 2020) and up to 15 months in women diagnosed with breast cancer (Swainston & Derakshan, 2018). Improvement on this training in high worriers and anxious individuals has correlated with increases in working memory capacity and reductions in worry (Course-Choi et al., 2017; Hotton et al., 2018) and anxiety (Sari et al., 2016). Therefore, improvement on the adaptive n-back training was shown to have both cognitive and affective transfer effects and might be particularly effective in vulnerable populations.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text