Evaluation of behavioral measures of risk taking propensity with inner city adolescents

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2003.12.007Get rights and content

Abstract

The current study examined the utility of behavioral measures of risk-taking propensity in the assessment of self-reported real-world risk-taking behaviors using a sample of 51 high-school-aged inner-city adolescents. Results indicated that performance on one behavioral measure, the balloon analogue risk task, accounted for unique variance in self-reported delinquency/safety risk behaviors as well as substance use risk behaviors, above and beyond that provided with demographics and self-report measures of risk-related constructs (i.e., impulsivity and sensation seeking). These results are discussed in relation to the potential utility of using a multimethod assessment approach for better understanding risk-taking vulnerability among adolescents.

Introduction

Risk-taking behaviors are those that involve potential negative consequences (Jessor, 1998), balanced in some way by perceived positive consequences (Gullone & Moore, 2000; Leigh, 1999). Available studies indicate that risk behaviors often are established during adolescence and remain as major contributors to the health problems of adults, including Axis I and II psychopathology as well as physical illnesses (Bachman et al., 1991; Barnes & Welte, 1986; Dembo, Williams, Schmeidler, & Berry, 1992; Resnick et al., 1997). Given the potential negative health outcomes associated with adolescent risk behaviors, the development of technologies for assessing a propensity to engage in risk-taking behaviors is of great public health importance.

One strategy for assessing risk-taking is to use self-report measures of related constructs such as impulsivity and sensation seeking (Brown, DiClemente, & Park, 1992; Neumark-Sztainer, Story, French, & Resnick, 1997; Gullone & Moore, 2000; Pack, Crosby, & St. Lawrence, 2001). Despite evidence suggesting the value of such an approach (Krueger et al., 2002; Sher, Bartholow, & Wood, 2000), evidence also suggests that these constructs alone are not sufficient to capture fully the multidimensional nature of risk taking (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1977; Wills, Sandy, & Yaeger, 2002). Additionally, relying exclusively on a unimethod assessment approach (i.e., self-report) has well-established limitations (e.g., method variance; Campbell & Stanley, 1963). In the case of adolescent risk taking, there also are some specific reasons why exclusively relying on self-report measures of risk-related constructs may be problematic. First, the veracity of self-reports may be affected by any perceived negative consequences of reporting risky behavior. Second, some respondents may lack the insight or cognitive ability to provide an accurate report of their own behavior (e.g., Ladouceur et al., 2000). Finally, because many of these instruments often rely on questions that directly query about the behavior under question, their use in early identification of risk behaviors for primary prevention purposes are limited (Andrew & Cronin, 1997).

Rather than reling exclusively on self-report measures, researchers have begun to develop and utilize behavioral assessment tools that allow for a controlled assessment of actual risk-taking behavior. Although engagement in any particular risk behavior does not guarantee engagement in other risk-taking behaviors, research does suggest some level of clustering among risk behaviors (Gullone & Moore, 2000; Jessor & Jessor, 1977; Kar, 1999; Lejuez et al., 2002), and therefore the utility of identifying a propensity to take risks.

To date, the Bechara gambling task (BGT; Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, 1994) has arguably been the most popular and well-tested behavioral measure of risk taking with adults. In this task, the participant is provided with four decks of cards on a computer screen. Using a mouse, the participant clicks on any of four decks. After each selection, the computer provides feedback indicating the amount of money the participant has won and/or lost on that card as well as a grand total. Following this feedback, the participant can select another card. For cards from two of the decks (A and B), the winnings are high but the losses are even higher. In contrast, for cards from the other two decks (C and D), the winnings are somewhat low but the losses are even lower. Thus, according to Bechara et al. (2001), decks A and B are “disadvantageous” while decks C and D are “advantageous”. In this way, risk taking may be indexed by the percentage of cards selected from the disadvantageous decks. Research using the BGT has focused on differentiating typologies of adults who engage in substance abuse. Several studies indicate that adult drug abusers may be more risky than nondrug abusing adults on the BGT (Bechara et al., 2001; Petry, 2001; Petry, Bickel, & Arnett, 1998), yet its appropriateness with adolescents has yet to be determined.

A second risk-taking task is the Balloon analogue risk task (BART; Lejuez et al., 2002). In this computerized task, participants accumulate money in a temporary bank by pressing a button that inflates a simulated balloon. Each balloon has an explosion point, which if reached, results in the loss of all money in the temporary bank. After each pump that does not result in an explosion, participants have the option of pressing a collect button that will transfer their money to a permanent bank. There are a set number of balloons, and regardless of whether the balloon explodes or money is collected, the participant moves on to the next balloon. Therefore, in deciding whether to make each pump, the participant must balance the potential gain of accruing more money against the potential risk of losing all money accrued for that balloon. Thus, unlike the BGT, in which each trial involves a choice between a risky and nonrisky alternative (i.e., card selection), the BART involves a variable number of choices in a context of increasing risk (i.e., the amount of money accrued and the probability of losing that money increase with each pump of the balloon).

In studies using young adults (age range: 18–25), the BART was shown to have convergent validity with measures of risk-related constructs such as sensation seeking, impulsivity, and deficiencies in behavioral constraint (Lejuez et al., 2002). Additionally, riskiness on the BART was correlated with the self-reported occurrence of addictive, delinquency, and safety risk behaviors, with the task accounting for variance in these behaviors beyond that accounted for by demographics and self-report measures of risk-related constructs (Lejuez et al., 2003, Lejuez et al., 2002). Despite these promising results, and similar to the BGT, its appropriateness with adolescents has yet to be determined.

Taken together, there is a need for behavioral assessment tools that can effectively identify risk-taking propensity in adolescents, especially in at-risk samples (Kalichman, 1998; Kar, 1999). Accordingly, the current study evaluated the relationship between two behavioral assessment measures and self-reported real-world risk-taking behavior among high school aged inner-city adolescents. Given the added resources necessary in using behavioral measures, we further examined their utility above and beyond that provided with self-report measures of risk-related constructs (i.e., impulsivity and sensation seeking). Specifically, we expected that impulsivity and sensation seeking would account for a modest degree of variance in self-reported risk taking behaviors, and that the behavioral tasks would evidence incremental validity.

Section snippets

Participants

Fifty-one, 9–12th grade adolescents, (M age=14.8; SD=1.5; 26 males and 25 females; 98% African American) completed a simple demographics form including age and gender (given the low-income sample targeted, measures of socioeconomic status were not collected), as well as a variety of questionnaires and behavioral risk tasks. They also completed a self-report measure modeled after the Center for Disease Control Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System (YRBSS; CDC, 2001) assessing engagement in

Relationships among the risk behaviors

As shown in the upper rows of Table 1, moderate intercorrelations were apparent among the risk behaviors. To reduce the number of dependent variables (and given the modest sample size), we used the recommended principal components analysis (PCA) with an oblique (promax) rotation to determine which risk behaviors could be combined into composite scores (Comrey, 1988). Examination of the screen plot indicated that the first two components accounted for the majority of variance in the measures.

Discussion

Although previous work has relied on self-report instruments to tap risk-taking propensity, behavioral tasks may be an important compliment to such assessment strategies for both conceptual and methodological reasons. Unfortunately, behavioral tasks rarely have been tested with adolescents. Therefore, the primary objective of the present investigation was to evaluate the incremental validity of two behavioral measures in the assessment of self-reported risk taking in an adolescent, inner-city

Acknowledgements

This research was conducted as part of the first author's requirement for a master's degree in clinical psychology, under the supervision of the second author. This work was supported by National Institute of Drug Abuse Grant R21 DA014699 awarded to the second author. The authors thank Samuel M. Turner, Mike Dougherty, Janie Johnson, Beverly E. Whitaker and N. Long for helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript, Timothy Jones, Eshe Raki Earl, Ezekiel Norman, and Gillian Pratt for

References (46)

  • G. Barnes et al.

    Patterns of predictors of alcohol use among 7–12th grade students in New York State

    Journal of Studies on Alcohol

    (1986)
  • J.P. Byrnes et al.

    Gender differences in risk takingA meta-analysis

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1999)
  • D.T. Campbell et al.

    Experimental and quasi-experimental research designs for research in teaching

  • Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance

    (2001)
  • A.L. Comrey

    Factor-analytic methods of scale development in personality and clinical psychology

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1988)
  • R. Dembo et al.

    A structural model examining the relationship between physical child abuse, sexual victimization, and marijuana/hashish use in delinquent youthA longitudinal study

    Violence & Victims

    (1992)
  • S.B.G. Eysenck et al.

    The place of impulsiveness in a dimensional system of personality description

    British Journal of Clinical and Social Psychology

    (1977)
  • G. Holmbeck

    A developmental perspective on adolescent health and illnessAn introduction to the special issues

    Journal of Pediatric Psychology

    (2002)
  • R. Jessor

    New perspectives on adolescent risk behavior

  • S.L. Jessor et al.

    Problem behavior and psychosocial developmentA longitudinal study of youth

    (1977)
  • S.C. Kalichman

    Preventing AIDS

    (1998)
  • S.B. Kar

    Substance use preventionA multicultural perspective

    (1999)
  • J.A. Kelly et al.

    Behavioral research in HIV/AIDS primary and secondary preventionRecent advances and future directions

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2002)
  • Cited by (243)

    • Computational markers of risky decision-making predict for relapse to alcohol

      2024, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text