Elsevier

Brain Research

Volume 1580, 11 September 2014, Pages 133-142
Brain Research

Research Report
Maternal oxytocin response predicts mother-to-infant gaze

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.10.050Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We examine association between maternal oxytocin response and mother-to-infant gaze.

  • Maternal oxytocin is positively associated with duration of mother-to-infant gaze.

  • Maternal oxytocin is negatively associated with maternal gaze shifts from infant.

  • Mothers with low/average oxytocin response reduce their gaze during infant distress.

  • Mothers with high oxytocin response maintain their gaze during infant distress.

Abstract

The neuropeptide oxytocin is importantly implicated in the emergence and maintenance of maternal behavior that forms the basis of the mother–infant bond. However, no research has yet examined the specific association between maternal oxytocin and maternal gaze, a key modality through which the mother makes social contact and engages with her infant. Furthermore, prior oxytocin studies have assessed maternal engagement primarily during episodes free of infant distress, while maternal engagement during infant distress is considered to be uniquely relevant to the formation of secure mother–infant attachment. Two patterns of maternal gaze, maternal gaze toward and gaze shifts away from the infant, were micro-coded while 50 mothers interacted with their 7-month-old infants during a modified still-face procedure. Maternal oxytocin response was defined as a change from baseline in the mother's plasma oxytocin level following interaction with her infant. The mother's oxytocin response was positively associated with the duration of time her gaze was directed toward her infant, while negatively associated with the frequency with which her gaze shifted away from her infant. Importantly, mothers who showed low/average oxytocin response demonstrated a significant decrease in their infant gaze during periods of infant distress, while such change was not observed in mothers with high oxytocin response. The findings underscore the involvement of oxytocin in regulating the mother's responsive engagement with her infant, particularly in times when the infant's need for access to the mother is greatest.

This article is part of a Special Issue entitled Oxytocin and Social Behav.

Introduction

While the neuropeptide oxytocin has been recognized for its functions in parturition and milk ejection for many decades, there exists now a substantial literature underscoring the role of oxytocin in regulating social behaviors (see reviews, Benarroch, 2013, Feldman, 2012, Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2011). A sizable number of studies have implicated oxytocin in maternal care (Febo et al., 2005, Pedersen et al., 2006, Strathearn et al., 2009b), pair bonding (Ross et al., 2009, Schneiderman et al., 2012), interpersonal trust (Van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2012), emotion recognition (Lischke et al., 2012, Perry et al., 2013), and empathy (Hurlemann et al., 2010, Rodrigues et al., 2009). Oxytocin has been characterized as a “hormone of affiliation” (Insel, 1992) and the oxytocinergic system has received attention as a key neural substrate of maternal caregiving, involved in the emergence and maintenance of maternal behaviors (Feldman, 2012, Strathearn, 2011). Many important advances in this regard have come from animal models (Francis et al., 2002, Keverne and Kendrick, 1992, Maestripieri et al., 2009, Pedersen et al., 2006, Williams et al., 2001), and they have been extended to human subjects over the past decade, revealing both parallel and divergent findings.

In humans, peripheral oxytocin levels are higher in pregnant and parturient women than non-pregnant women (Feldman et al., 2007, Gordon et al., 2008). Oxytocin levels show high intra-individual stability over the course of pregnancy (Feldman et al., 2007, Levine et al., 2007) and early motherhood (Gordon et al., 2010), suggesting that they may constitute a trait-like characteristic that underpins the expression of maternal behavior. Prospective and cross-sectional studies have demonstrated that maternal oxytocin levels are systematically associated with naturally occurring variations in maternal behavior, with high plasma oxytocin levels during pregnancy and postpartum predicting increased maternal behavior in the postpartum months (Atzil et al., 2011, Feldman et al., 2007, Gordon et al., 2010). Interaction with their young in the postpartum period further stimulates oxytocin response in mothers (Feldman et al., 2010a, Feldman et al., 2010b), though significant inter-individual variations have been found (Strathearn et al., 2012), as with the baseline oxytocin levels. These natural variations in maternal oxytocin response have systematically predicted differences in the quality of maternal care provided by mothers (Feldman et al., 2010a, Feldman et al., 2010b).

Quality provision of maternal care and formation of secure attachment bonds are of particular importance in the early postpartum months, given their long-term effects on the development of the offspring (Fonagy et al., 2007, Kochanska and Kim, 2013, Sroufe et al., 2005, Weinfield et al., 2004). It is well established that sensitive and responsive maternal behavior has direct bearings on the child's life-long capacity for social adaptation and stress regulation (Kochanska, 2001, Mayes, 2006, Mikulincer and Shaver, 2007, Schore, 2001). One important channel through which maternal sensitive responsiveness is communicated is mother-to-infant gaze. Gaze is a central modality through which mothers signal their availability, establish mutual engagement, and initiate regulation of infant arousal, particularly in times of infant distress (Beebe et al., 2010, Slee, 1984). Infants are highly sensitive to their mothers' gaze (Stern, 1974) and begin to join in mutual gaze with their mothers as early as 3 months of age, which serves as a basis for the mother–infant synchrony that subsequently emerges in other modalities (e.g., touch, vocalization, facial expression; Colonnesi et al., 2012, Feldman, 2007, Lavelli and Fogel, 2013, Tronick et al., 1980).

Despite its significance, the specific association between maternal gaze and maternal oxytocin has not yet been examined. Previous studies have measured maternal gaze, but only as part of a composite of maternal behaviors (also encompassing touch, vocalization, and affect; e.g., Atzil et al., 2011, Feldman et al., 2007, Feldman et al., 2010b, Gordon et al., 2010). Furthermore, while attachment literature has underscored that maternal sensitivity to infant distress uniquely contributes to optimal socioemotional outcomes in the child (Leerkes et al., 2009, McElwain and Booth-LaForce, 2006), prior studies have examined maternal oxytocin only in relation to indices of maternal synchrony during episodes of positive affect and have not considered episodes of distress (Atzil et al., 2011, Feldman et al., 2010b, Feldman et al., 2011).

In our previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study (Strathearn et al., 2009b), we demonstrated that mothers' peripheral oxytocin responses predicted their blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) brain responses to their own infants' faces. The greater the mothers' oxytocin responses during interactions with their infants, the greater was their activation in regions known to be rich in oxytocin receptors (i.e., hypothalamic/pituitary region) when viewing their infants' faces in the scanner. In the present study, we extended our line of investigation to the mother's actual gaze behavior during real-time interaction with her infant. We sought to examine the relationship between maternal oxytocin response and mother-to-infant gaze during periods of infant non-distress as well as distress.

Mother–infant dyads were observed during 50-min semi-structured interaction sessions. Maternal oxytocin response was defined as any change in the mother's oxytocin level following interaction with her infant compared to baseline. Two patterns of maternal gaze, duration of maternal gaze toward and frequency of gaze shifts away from the infant, were coded during a well-validated interaction paradigm, a modified still-face procedure (MSFP; Koos and Gergely, 2001). The MSFP is a three-phase procedure, during which the mother interacts freely with the infant in phases 1 and 3, but is instructed to maintain a neutral ‘still face’ during phase 2, suddenly depriving the infant of maternal contingency and inducing stress in the infant (Koos and Gergely, 2001, Tronick et al., 1978; Fig. 1). The experimental manipulation reliably produces changes in the infant's level of distress: infants display clear signs of distress during phase 2, which have been shown to carry over to phase 3 (Haley and Stansbury, 2003, Mesman et al., 2009). The MSFP thereby offers an opportunity to examine the mother's behavior in the absence and presence of signals of infant distress. We hypothesized that maternal oxytocin response would be positively associated with maternal gaze toward the infant, and negatively associated with maternal gaze shifts away from the infant. We further predicted that this association would become more pronounced during periods of infant distress than non-distress, in phase 3 compared with phase 1 of the MSFP.

Section snippets

Participant characteristics and preliminary analyses

Participant characteristics are shown in Table 1. Participants were a non-clinical sample of first-time mothers who were generally of middle to high socioeconomic status, three-quarters of whom held a college or postgraduate degree. All mothers scored below the clinical range for personality disorders, while four mothers scored in the mildly depressed range, one of whom also scored in the clinically significant range for parenting stress. However, these four mothers did not differ from the rest

Discussion

The present results are the first, to our knowledge, to document that measures of maternal peripheral oxytocin are systematically associated with individual variations in mother-to-infant gaze. As hypothesized, maternal peripheral oxytocin response was positively associated with the duration of mother-to-infant gaze, while negatively associated with the frequency with which maternal gaze was directed away from infants. Also consistent with our expectation, these associations were more

Participants

Participants were 50 first-time mothers aged 19–41 (M=28.0±.7) years, recruited through prenatal clinics and community advertisements as part of a larger study. Of 116 participants initially recruited during the third trimester of pregnancy, 61 met eligibility criteria, and 50 completed oxytocin sampling and MSFP at 7 months postpartum. Exclusion criterion included mothers with past or present alcohol or substance abuse, mothers who used cigarettes during pregnancy, and mothers who were on

Funding sources

This work was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [K23 HD43097, R01 HD065819]; General Clinical Research Center [MO1 RR00188]; the Baylor Child Health Research Center: Pediatrics Mentored Research Program [K12 HD41648]; and the National Institute on Drug Abuse [R01 DA026437]. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of these institutes or the National Institutes of

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Houming Cai and the late Dr. Janet Amico for assistance in performing the radioimmunoassays of oxytocin. Udita Iyengar and Sheila Martinez provided valuable assistance with data management and analysis.

References (73)

  • D. Maestripieri

    Mother–infant interactions in free-ranging rhesus macaques: relationships between physiological and behavioral variables

    Physiol. Beh.

    (2009)
  • M.E. McCullough et al.

    Problems with measuring peripheral oxytocin: can the data on oxytocin and human behavior be trusted?

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2013)
  • J. Mesman et al.

    The many faces of the still-face paradigm: a review and meta-analysis

    Dev. Rev.

    (2009)
  • I. Schneiderman

    Oxytocin during the initial stages of romantic attachment: relations to couples' interactive reciprocity

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2012)
  • P.T. Slee

    The nature of mother–infant gaze patterns during interaction as a function of emotional expression

    J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry

    (1984)
  • C.T. Snowdon

    Variation in oxytocin is related to variation in affiliative behavior in monogamous, pairbonded tamarins

    Horm. Beh.

    (2010)
  • L. Strathearn

    Maternal oxytocin response during mother–infant interaction: associations with adult temperament

    Horm. Behav.

    (2012)
  • E. Tronick

    The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction

    J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry

    (1978)
  • M.H. Van IJzendoorn et al.

    A sniff of trust: meta-analysis of the effects of intranasal oxytocin administration on face recognition, trust to in-group, and trust to out-group

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2012)
  • R. Abidin

    Parenting Stress Index

    Professional Manual

    (1995)
  • J.G. Allen

    Mentalizing in the Development and Treatment of Attachment Trauma

    (2013)
  • J.A. Amico

    A novel oxytocin-like and vasotocin-like peptide in human plasma after administration of estrogen

    J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab.

    (1985)
  • J.A. Amico et al.

    Pattern of oxytocin concentrations in the plasma and cerebrospinal fluid of lactating rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): evidence for functionally independent oxytocinergic pathways in primates

    J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab.

    (1990)
  • S. Atzil et al.

    Specifying the neurobiological basis of human attachment: brain, hormones, and behavior in synchronous and intrusive mothers

    Neuropsychopharmacology

    (2011)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory II

    (1996)
  • B. Beebe

    The origins of 12-month attachment: a microanalysis of 4-month mother–infant interaction

    Attach. Hum. Dev.

    (2010)
  • E.E. Benarroch

    Oxytocin and vasopressin: social neuropeptides with complex neuromodulatory functions

    Neurology

    (2013)
  • J. Bowlby

    Attachment

    (1969/1982)
  • C.S. Carter

    Oxytocin: behavioral associations and potential as a salivary biomarker

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2007)
  • F. Champagne

    Naturally occurring variations in maternal behavior in the rat are associated with differences in estrogen-inducible central oxytocin receptors

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (2001)
  • F.A. Champagne

    Natural variations in maternal care are associated with estrogen receptor alpha expression and estrogen sensitivity in the medial preoptic area

    Endocrinology

    (2003)
  • F.A. Champagne

    Maternal care associated with methylation of the estrogen receptor-alpha1b promoter and estrogen receptor-alpha expression in the medial preoptic area of female offspring

    Endocrinology

    (2006)
  • M.Y. Dawood

    Oxytocin in maternal circulation and amniotic fluid during pregnancy

    J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab.

    (1979)
  • K. de Geest

    Plasma oxytocin in human pregnancy and parturition

    J. Perinat. Med.

    (1985)
  • M. Febo et al.

    Functional magnetic resonance imaging shows oxytocin activates brain regions associated with mother–pup bonding during suckling

    J. Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • R. Feldman

    Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions

    J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl.

    (2007)
  • Cited by (0)

    1

    The author is now at Child Trauma Training Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Massachusetts Medical School, 76 Church Street, Suite 301, Whitinsville, MA 01588.

    View full text