Elsevier

Brain Research

Volume 1340, 22 June 2010, Pages 40-51
Brain Research

Research Report
Effective connectivities of cortical regions for top-down face processing: A Dynamic Causal Modeling study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.044Get rights and content

Abstract

To study top-down face processing, the present study used an experimental paradigm in which participants detected non-existent faces in pure noise images. Conventional BOLD signal analysis identified three regions involved in this illusory face detection. These regions included the left orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) in addition to the right fusiform face area (FFA) and right occipital face area (OFA), both of which were previously known to be involved in both top-down and bottom-up processing of faces. We used Dynamic Causal Modeling (DCM) and Bayesian model selection to further analyze the data, revealing both intrinsic and modulatory effective connectivities among these three cortical regions. Specifically, our results support the claim that the orbitofrontal cortex plays a crucial role in the top-down processing of faces by regulating the activities of the occipital face area, and the occipital face area in turn detects the illusory face features in the visual stimuli and then provides this information to the fusiform face area for further analysis.

Introduction

Humans are exceptionally skilled at face perception. Over a wide range of viewing conditions we effortlessly detect and recognize faces accurately and quickly. Traditionally, face processing is viewed as a feed-forward, bottom-up process in which facial identity is processed first in the ventral visual stream and then passed along to more anterior regions of the brain such as the frontal cortex. This face processing model has been supported by many existing studies. However, in these experiments the stimuli are high quality images of actual faces, maximizing bottom-up information. This may swamp any top-down influences on face perception, thus biasing interpretation of experimental results in favor of bottom-up driven processing models.

Providing evidence that supports the bidirectional interactive face processing model proposed by Haxby et al. (2000), recent studies using novel stimuli and paradigms have questioned the traditional view about the neural systems of face processing. These studies focused on top-down face processing by requiring participants to imagine faces (Ishai et al., 2000, Mechelli et al., 2004), or to interpret ambiguous face stimuli such as Mooney faces and vase/face illusion (Andrews & Schluppeck, 2004, Hasson et al., 2001), or to detect impoverished face stimuli (Gosselin & Schyns, 2001, Summerfield et al., 2006, Wild & Busey, 2004), or to even make illusory face detection of pure visual noise stimuli (Li et al., 2009, Liu et al., 2010, Zhang et al., 2008). It was found that top-down feed-backward mechanisms play an important role in face processing, which is perhaps engendered by the neural system's use of face-relevant knowledge and learned expectations that regulate the bottom-up processing of visual stimuli (Mechelli et al., 2004, Summerfield et al., 2006). Further, these studies revealed a distributed cortical network for top-down face processing (Li et al., 2009), which overlaps to a large extent with the face processing network reported in traditional bottom-up face processing studies (for reviews, see Haxby et al., 2000, Ishai et al., 2005, Ishai, 2008). Taken together, the findings from bottom-up and top-down paradigms suggest that the neural system for processing faces involves a network of neural regions distributed from occipital to frontal cortices that has both feed-forward and feed-backward connections (Fairhall & Ishai, 2007, Haxby et al., 2000, Ishai, 2008, Li et al., 2009, Mechelli et al., 2004, Summerfield et al., 2006).

Previous studies of top-down illusory face processing (Li et al., 2009, Zhang et al., 2008) used conventional analyses that cannot determine in which direction signals flow and whether connections between brain regions are modulated during the task. The term “effective connectivity” is used in referring to the connection strength between different brain regions and how these strengths vary with experimental manipulations. To understand the top-down face processing network in terms of effective connectivity, the analyses reported here used Dynamic Causal Modeling (DCM: Friston et al., 2003). This analysis not only determines the active neural connections between brain regions during the experiment, but it also determines the direction of the intrinsic and modulatory cortical pathways specifically involved in top-down face perception.

The present study focused on three cortical regions identified by traditional analyses: the fusiform face area (FFA), occipital face areas (OFA), and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). It is now well established that the FFA and OFA play an important role in face processing and are part of the bottom-up and top-down face processing networks (Fairhall & Ishai, 2007, Liu et al., 2009, Mechelli et al., 2004, Summerfield et al., 2006, Zhang et al., 2009). Thus, these are “core regions” of face processing. However, the function of the OFC and its relation to the core face processing regions is less clear.

A number of functional neuroimaging studies have identified OFC activation both during face processing and during processing of non-face objects. It has been proposed that the OFC is involved in encoding novel information (Frey & Petrides, 2000, Frey et al., 2004) as well as in mediating the perception of attractive and sexually relevant faces (Ishai, 2007, Kranz & Ishai, 2006, O'Doherty et al., 2001). More importantly, recent studies of object recognition revealed that the OFC plays a key role in top-down object processing (Bar et al., 2006, Bar, 2009, Johnson, 2005, Kveraga et al., 2007a, Kveraga et al., 2007b). Specifically, it has been proposed that the OFC uses low spatial frequency visual information to form a coarse prediction of the most likely candidate object, which is used to prime the corresponding object processing areas in the ventral occipital-temporal cortex in a top-down manner. This hypothesis predicts that during top-down face detection, the OFC should have functional connections to the OFA or the FFA. The present study tested this prediction.

Based on our recent investigation on top-down face processing (Li et al., 2009, Zhang et al., 2008), we adopted a novel paradigm that promotes illusory face detection in response to images that only contain noise. Participants were told that half of the images in the experiment contained faces and the other half did not. Their task was to detect which images contained faces. An initial training stage of the experiment did indeed contain faces on 50% of the trials. During this training, the faces became more and more difficult to detect by mixing higher degrees of noise with the faces. Eventually, participants were only shown pure noise images, although they were instructed that there were still faces on 50% of the trials and that face detection would be very difficult. The noise images were a mixture of Gaussian blobs of different spatial frequencies placed randomly throughout the image. These complex noise images lend themselves to a large number of interpretations and participants readily continued to detect faces. Thus, we were able to study top-down influences using false detections of faces to images containing only noise, which avoids contamination from strong bottom-up face information. Furthermore, an independent localizer task was performed to validate the ventral occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas identified by the illusory face detection task. Our recent study using this method revealed a complex distributed cortical network for top-down face processing (Li et al., 2009). However, this finding was obtained by using simple correlational analyses (Psychophysiological Interaction, or PPI) with the right FFA as the seed region. Not only is this method unable to measure the directional effective connectivity between different brain regions involved in top-down face processing, but this method also has potential methodological problems such as “double dipping” in which the same data is used for more than one analysis (see Kriegeskorte et al., 2009).

To determine the directional top-down effective connectivities involved in the top-down face processing as well as to avoid the methodological problems associated with PPI, we used DCM (Friston et al., 2003) in combination with Bayesian model selection (Penny et al., 2004) to analyze the data obtained in Li et al. (2009). The use of DCM has several advantages in addition to avoiding double dipping (Stephan et al., 2010). First, this analytic method provides information not only about the intrinsic effective connectivities among various brain regions (i.e., connections strengths that are constant throughout the experiment), but it also provides information about enhanced connectivities due to a specific processing demand (i.e., illusory face detection). Rather than revealing simple correlational relationships, DCM extracts directional relationships, providing information about how different brain regions are functionally connected during object processing (e.g. in a feed-forward or feed-backward manner). This information is particularly important for the present study considering that we were interested in the interplay between the FFA, OFA and OFC in top-down face processing.

Section snippets

Behavioral results

The average proportion of trials on which subjects responded “face” was 34%, with a standard deviation (SD) of 14%, across the 480 pure noise images. The mean reaction times of the “face” and “no face” responses were 723 ms (SD = 126 ms) and 698 ms (SD = 119 ms) respectively, which are not significantly different from each other (t(10) = 1.6, p = 0.169).

Conventional fMRI analysis

In the localizer task, in the right hemisphere, all twelve subjects showed activation in the right middle fusiform gyrus and the right lateral occipital

Discussion

The present study examined illusory face detection to pure noise images to investigate the neural networks involved in top-down face processing. Consistent with previous findings using PPI (Li et al., 2009), conventional BOLD signal analysis identified three core brain regions, namely the FFA, OFA, and OFC, that were highly responsive to trials on which participants detected a face. Focusing on these three regions, application of Bayesian model selection to Dynamic Causal Modeling determined

Subjects

Twelve normal, right-handed subjects (seven males, age 23.8 ± 1.4 years), with normal or corrected-to-normal vision, participated in this study. All subjects gave written informed consent for the procedure in accordance with protocols approved by the Human Research Protection Program of Tiantan Hospital, Beijing, China.

Design and procedure

The experiment included two stages: an initial training stage and an illusory face detection task stage (Fig. 3C). Four types of stimuli were used: face images overlaid with 50%

Acknowledgments

This paper is supported by the Joint Research Fund for Overseas Chinese Young Scholars under Grant No. 30528027, the National Natural Science Foundation of China under Grant Nos. 60910006, 30873462, 30970769, 30970771, 30873462, 30870685, 60621001, 30970774, 60901064, 60902083, the Chair Professors of Cheung Kong Scholars Program of Ministry of Education of China, CAS Hundred Talents Program, Changjiang Scholars and Innovative Research Team in University (PCSIRT) under Grant No. IRT0645, the

References (51)

  • A. Ishai

    Sex, beauty and the orbitofrontal cortex

    Int. J. Psychophysiol.

    (2007)
  • A. Ishai

    Let's face it: it's a cortical network

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • A. Ishai et al.

    Distributed neural systems for the generation of visual images

    Neuron

    (2000)
  • A. Ishai et al.

    Face perception is mediated by a distributed cortical network

    Brain Res. Bull.

    (2005)
  • F. Kranz et al.

    Face perception is modulated by sexual preference

    Curr. Biol.

    (2006)
  • K. Kveraga et al.

    Top-down predictions in the cognitive brain

    Brain Cogn.

    (2007)
  • J. Li et al.

    A distributed neural system for top-down face processing

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2009)
  • J. Liu et al.

    Neural correlates of top-down letter processing

    Neuropsychologia

    (2010)
  • W.D. Penny et al.

    Comparing dynamic causal models

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • D. Pitcher et al.

    TMS evidence for the involvement of the right occipital face area in early face processing

    Curr. Biol.

    (2007)
  • C. Schiltz et al.

    Faces are represented holistically in the human occipito-temporal cortex

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • K.E. Stephan et al.

    Comparing hemodynamic models with DCM

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • K.E. Stephan et al.

    Ten simple rules for dynamic causal modeling

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • H. Zhang et al.

    Intrinsically organized network for face perception during the resting state

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2009)
  • T. Allison et al.

    Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I: Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli

    Cereb. Cortex

    (1999)
  • Cited by (42)

    • Mental imagery of face enhances face-sensitive event-related potentials to ambiguous visual stimuli

      2017, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      It has been suggested that imagining visual objects in the mind may activate corresponding brain areas for perception of visual objects (O’Craven & Kanwisher, 2000; Klein et al., 2004). For example, some functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have shown that imagery of illusory faces is associated with activation of multiple cortical regions of the visual system, including the fusiform face area (FFA) and the occipital face area (OFA) (Li et al., 2010; Nestor et al., 2013; Righart et al., 2009), which are also the brain areas specific for face perception (Kanwisher, McDermott, & Chun, 1997; Haxby, Hoffman, & Gobbini, 2000). Extended brain regions for face imagery may also involve the orbitofrontal cortex (Li et al., 2010), inferior frontal gyri (Zhang et al., 2008), and a distributed network between frontal and occipito-temporal areas (Liu et al., 2014).

    • Mechanisms of hemispheric lateralization: Asymmetric interhemispheric recruitment in the face perception network

      2016, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Nevertheless, such a mechanistic understanding of the network dynamics is crucial for unraveling how the human brain processes faces, and might provide new insights into the pathophysiology of diseases where face perception is impaired (e.g., prosopagnosia, autism). Only lately have pioneering studies begun to address the effective connectivity (i.e., directed interactions) among face-sensitive regions (Cohen Kadosh et al., 2011; Dima et al., 2011; Ewbank et al., 2013; Fairhall and Ishai, 2007; Ishai, 2008; Li et al., 2010). These studies, however, have only examined intrahemispheric connections, while neglecting the interhemispheric connections of the network.

    • Test-retest reliability of dynamic causal modeling for fMRI

      2015, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Specifically, DCM focuses on how directed interactions among brain regions are perturbed by experimental manipulations (e.g., sensory stimulation, task demands). Using DCM, researchers have gained deeper insight into the mechanisms underlying cognitive tasks, such as visuospatial attention (Kellermann et al., 2012; Siman-Tov et al., 2007), face perception (Fairhall and Ishai, 2007; Li et al., 2010; Nguyen et al., 2014), working memory (Dima et al., 2014), decision making (Stephan et al., 2007a; Summerfield et al., 2006; Summerfield and Koechlin, 2008), motor processes (Grefkes et al., 2008; Grol et al., 2007), and the “resting state” (Goulden et al., 2014; Di and Biswal, 2014; Friston et al., 2014). In this regard, research questions have become increasingly complex, leading to ongoing refinements and extensions of DCM for fMRI, such as (i) two-state DCM (Marreiros et al., 2008), allowing for two neuronal states per brain region to model the activity of inhibitory and excitatory populations; (ii) nonlinear DCM (nlDCM; Stephan et al., 2008), which accounts for synaptic gating; i.e., the modulatory influence of a neuronal population on the connection between two other populations; and (iii) stochastic DCM (sDCM; Daunizeau et al., 2009; Friston et al., 2008, 2010), that allows for endogenous (stochastic) fluctuations at the neuronal level.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text