Elsevier

Brain Research

Volume 1262, 25 March 2009, Pages 48-63
Brain Research

Research Report
Aberrant functional connectivity in autism: Evidence from low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.12.076Get rights and content

Abstract

A number of recent studies have examined functional connectivity in individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD), generally converging on the finding of reduced interregional coordination, or underconnectivity. Underconnectivity has been reported between many brain regions and across a range of cognitive tasks, and has been proposed to underlie behavioral and cognitive impairments associated with ASD. The current study employed functional connectivity MRI (fcMRI) to examine interregional correlations of low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations in 10 high-functioning participants with ASD and 10 typically developing control participants. Whole-brain connectivity with three seed regions of interest (left middle frontal, left superior parietal, and left middle occipital cortex) was evaluated using fMRI datasets acquired during performance of a source recognition task. While fcMRI patterns were found to be largely similar across the two groups, including many common areas, effects for the ASD group were generally more extensive. These findings, although inconsistent with generalized underconnectivity in ASD, are compatible with a model of aberrant connectivity in which the nature of connectivity disturbance (i.e., increased or reduced) may vary by region. Taking into consideration methodological factors that might influence measured fcMRI effects, we suggest that ASD is associated with an inefficiency in optimizing network connections to achieve task performance.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are a group of complex neurodevelopmental disorders defined by impairments in social, behavioral, and communicative functioning (APA, 2000). The precise nature of the neuropathology in ASD is not fully understood. MRI and postmortem studies have identified cellular and volumetric abnormalities of numerous brain regions (Rapin and Katzman, 1998, Sokol and Edwards-Brown, 2004, Trottier et al., 1999), yet with the exception of the relatively consistent finding of early brain overgrowth (Courchesne et al., 2001, Hazlett et al., 2005, Sparks et al., 2002), there is little consensus regarding primary neuroanatomical disturbance. Functional neuroimaging investigations have been similarly inconsistent, with reports of atypical brain response in a variety of areas. Nonetheless, evidence of aberrant localization, intensity, and variability of neural activity in ASD suggests widely disrupted functional brain organization (Cody et al., 2002, Courchesne et al., 2004).

Inconsistent reports of regional brain abnormalities are not particularly surprising given the developmental nature of ASD. It is a strongly genetic disorder marked by atypical early brain growth (Courchesne et al., 2001, Courchesne et al., 2003), abnormal patterns of white matter development (Courchesne et al., 2001), and impairment in numerous cognitive domains by age three (APA, 2000). Such early disturbances undoubtedly alter developmental trajectories for afflicted individuals in diverse and complex ways, and would therefore not be predicted to have circumscribed neural effects. Rather, effects would be widespread, reflecting the ongoing interplay of pathology, normal maturational processes, and experience. Increasingly, research into the neural bases of ASD is moving away from explanations of isolated brain disturbances toward characterization of alterations in neural circuitry.

A number of recent studies have assessed anatomical connectivity in ASD through examination of white matter volume and integrity. On the whole, evidence from volumetric studies suggests atypical white matter growth patterns. That is, the relationship between age and white matter volume is abnormal, such that enlarged volume may be observed relative to typically developing individuals at one age, with relative reductions in volume or no group differences noted at other ages (Carper et al., 2002, Courchesne et al., 2001, Courchesne et al., 2003, Herbert et al., 2003, Herbert et al., 2004). One of the most commonly reported regions of white matter abnormality is the corpus callosum, which has been found to be smaller in ASD in several studies (Boger-Megiddo et al., 2006, Chung et al., 2004, Piven et al., 1997, Vidal et al., 2006, Waiter et al., 2005). Disturbance of this major pathway for interhemispheric information transfer provides compelling evidence of altered connectivity in ASD (Just et al., 2007, Kana et al., 2006). Additional evidence of aberrant anatomical connectivity has emerged from diffusion tensor imaging and transverse relaxation time imaging, which evaluate the integrity rather than volume of cerebral white matter. While the majority of studies employing these methodologies have demonstrated reduced white matter integrity in ASD (Alexander et al., 2007, Barnea-Goraly et al., 2004, Hendry et al., 2006), recent findings from Ben Bashat et al. (2007) suggest that very young children with autism (1.8–3.3 years of age) may show precocious axonal development and myelination.

In addition to investigations of anatomical connectivity, information about patterns of functional connectivity will likely be necessary to further understand the profile of cognitive and behavioral impairments associated with ASD. In particular, functional connectivity approaches can help characterize the neural basis of higher-order integrative processes, believed to be particularly impaired in this population (Hill and Frith, 2003, Minshew et al., 1997, Minshew et al., 2002). Functional connectivity MRI (fcMRI) assesses the correlation of BOLD signal fluctuations across brain regions and is based on the observation that interacting regions demonstrate similar BOLD signal profiles (Biswal et al., 1995, Hampson et al., 2002, Xiong et al., 1999). When measured during task performance, functional connectivity may reveal networks of regions coordinating to meet the particular cognitive demands of the task. Resting-state functional connectivity measures, on the other hand, are considered less context-dependent, and have the potential to reveal widespread neuroanatomical networks in the human brain (see Fox and Raichle, 2007 for review). Although several recent studies have investigated task-related functional connectivity in ASD (Just et al., 2004, Just et al., 2007, Kana et al., 2006, Kana et al., 2007, Kleinhans et al., 2008, Koshino et al., 2005, Koshino et al., 2008, Welchew et al., 2005), predominantly showing reduced functional connectivity, or “underconnectivity”, evidence of altered connectivity associated with task-free conditions remains limited (Cherkassky et al., 2006, Kennedy and Courchesne, 2008).

Some functional domains, such as theory of mind and face processing, have been extensively studied in ASD with neuroimaging techniques. Much less is known, however, about the functional organization of memory in this population. Within the behavioral literature, numerous studies have reported impairment of higher-order memory abilities. For example, across a series of studies, Minshew and colleagues have shown that while simple memory abilities are typically intact, memory for more complex material is often compromised (Minshew et al., 1997, Minshew and Goldstein, 1998, Minshew and Goldstein, 2001, Williams et al., 2006). Furthermore, episodic memory has been found to be impaired in ASD relative to control participants when task performance is aided by spontaneous use of organizational strategies (Bennetto et al., 1996, Minshew and Goldstein, 1993, Minshew and Goldstein, 2001, Tager-Flusberg, 1991). This finding likely reflects impaired higher-order integrative processes in ASD, on which spontaneous organization of items in memory relies. Source memory, a component of episodic memory referring to the ability to recall from which source and in which context an item was encoded into memory, has not been widely studied in ASD. Results from the few published studies have been mixed, providing inconsistent evidence of source memory deficits (Bennetto et al., 1996, Bowler et al., 2004, Hala et al., 2005, O'Shea et al., 2005).

The present study examined functional connectivity in ASD and control participants in the context of a source memory task. Source memory judgments required memory for single words and their encoding context (whether the word had been encountered in the auditory or visual modality). This task is well suited to detect regions involved in higher-order integrative functions, as performance relies on coordination between distributed brain regions to evaluate and integrate information presented across the two modalities. Three regions, identified on the basis of conventional activation analyses, were chosen as seed volumes for functional connectivity analyses. The three seed regions showed distinct profiles of source memory related activation across the two groups; one was engaged during task performance by control participants only (left middle frontal gyrus), one by ASD participants only (left middle occipital gyrus), and one by both groups (left superior parietal lobule).

Section snippets

Behavioral performance

To characterize memory performance, six possible response types were examined: correct source attribution responses (correct identification of the study modality of previously encountered words), correct rejections (correct identification of unstudied words as “new”), source attribution errors (incorrect identification of study modality), false positive errors (incorrect identification of a “new” word as previously studied), miss responses (incorrect identification of a studied word as “new”),

General remarks

The present study aimed to further characterize the neural bases of complex integrative functions and their breakdown in ASD through evaluating whole-brain fcMRI patterns in 10 high-functioning participants with ASD and 10 typically developing control participants. Three seed regions for functional connectivity analyses were identified from conventional activation analysis of a source memory dataset. One region showed significant response during source memory performance in the control group

Participants

Ten high-functioning male ASD participants (six diagnosed with autism, four with Asperger's Disorder) and 10 healthy male comparison participants were tested. ASD diagnoses were based on criteria from the DSM-IV (APA, 2000), Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Lord et al., 1994), and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000). ASD participants meeting criteria for Autistic Disorder on each of the three diagnostic measures were diagnosed with autism. A diagnosis of

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institutes of Health grants DC006155 and NS042639.

References (84)

  • FoxM.D. et al.

    Intrinsic fluctuations within cortical systems account for intertrial variability in human behavior

    Neuron

    (2007)
  • FranssonP.

    How default is the default mode of brain function? Further evidence from intrinsic BOLD signal fluctuations

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • HendryJ. et al.

    White matter abnormalities in autism detected through transverse relaxation time imaging

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • KanaR.K. et al.

    Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2007)
  • KennedyD.P. et al.

    The intrinsic functional organization of the brain is altered in autism

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • KoshinoH. et al.

    Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high-functioning autism

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • LoweM.J. et al.

    Correlations in low-frequency BOLD fluctuations reflect cortico-cortical connections

    Neuroimage

    (2000)
  • MizunoA. et al.

    Partially enhanced thalamocortical functional connectivity in autism

    Brain Res.

    (2006)
  • NirY. et al.

    Widespread functional connectivity and fMR1 fluctuations in human visual cortex in the absence of visual stimulation

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • RuggM.D. et al.

    The role of the prefrontal cortex in recognition memory and memory for source: an fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (1999)
  • VidalC.N. et al.

    Mapping corpus callosum deficits in autism: an index of aberrant cortical connectivity

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • WaiterG.D. et al.

    Structural white matter deficits in high-functioning individuals with autistic spectrum disorder: a voxel-based investigation

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • WelchewD.E. et al.

    Functional disconnectivity of the medial temporal lobe in Asperger's syndrome

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • WildingE.L.

    Separating retrieval strategies from retrieval success: an event-related potential study of source memory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1999)
  • Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Text Revision)

    (2000)
  • BelmonteM.K. et al.

    Autism as a disorder of neural information processing: directions for research and targets for therapy

    Mol. Psychiatry

    (2004)
  • BennettoL. et al.

    Intact and impaired memory functions in autism

    Child Dev.

    (1996)
  • Bianciardi, M., Fukunaga, M., van Gelderen, P., Horovitz, S.G., de Zwart, J.A., Duyn, J.H., in press. Modulation of...
  • BiswalB. et al.

    Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI

    Magn. Reson. Med.

    (1995)
  • Boger-MegiddoI. et al.

    Corpus callosum morphometrics in young children with autism spectrum disorder

    J. Autism. Dev. Disord.

    (2006)
  • BowlerD.M. et al.

    Source memory in adolescents and adults with Asperger's syndrome

    J. Autism. Dev. Disord.

    (2004)
  • BucknerR.L. et al.

    The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease

    Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2008)
  • CansinoS. et al.

    Brain activity underlying encoding and retrieval of source memory

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2002)
  • CherkasskyV.L. et al.

    Functional connectivity in a baseline resting-state network in autism

    Neuroreport

    (2006)
  • ColtheartM.

    The MRC Psycholinguistics Database

    Q. J. Exp. Psychol.

    (1981)
  • CordesD. et al.

    Mapping functionally related regions of brain with functional connectivity MR imaging

    AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol.

    (2000)
  • CourchesneE. et al.

    Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder: an MRI study

    Neurology

    (2001)
  • CourchesneE. et al.

    Evidence of brain overgrowth in the first year of life in autism

    JAMA

    (2003)
  • CourchesneE. et al.

    The autistic brain: birth through adulthood

    Curr. Opin. Neurol.

    (2004)
  • CoxR.W. et al.

    Software tools for analysis and visualization of fMRI data

    NMR Biomed.

    (1997)
  • DobbinsI.G. et al.

    fMRI evidence for separable and lateralized prefrontal memory monitoring processes

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • FormanS.D. et al.

    Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold

    Magn. Reson. Med.

    (1995)
  • Cited by (144)

    • Brain function and brain connectivity in autism

      2022, The Neuroscience of Autism
    • Gray matter abnormalities follow non-random patterns of co-alteration in autism: Meta-connectomic evidence

      2021, NeuroImage: Clinical
      Citation Excerpt :

      Converging lines of research offer insight into the link between the cerebellum and ASD symptomatology (Becker and Stoodley, 2013), showing the crucial role of the posterior right lobules and their circuits in core deficits, such as those in language and social cognition processing, stereotyped behaviors and impairments in imitation planning and affective regulation (D'Mello and Stoodley, 2015). Our meta-connectomic analysis accords well with these evidences, suggesting a tendency of the Crus2_R to be co-altered with a set of contralateral fronto-posterior nodes (i.e., IFG, MFG, MOG and PCUN) structurally connected and intrinsically implicated in an abnormal cerebro-cerebellar connectivity in mentalizing, verb generation, attention or resting-state tasks (D'Mello and Stoodley, 2015; Ecker et al., 2010; Itahashi et al., 2015; Khan et al., 2015; Noonan et al., 2009; Verly et al., 2014). The mechanisms that might explain the observed co-alterations are varied.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text