Elsevier

Biological Psychology

Volume 132, February 2018, Pages 1-8
Biological Psychology

Research Paper
Your attention makes me smile: Direct gaze elicits affiliative facial expressions

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.11.001Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Facial EMG and autonomic responses were measured to another individual’s direct and averted gaze.

  • Greater zygomatic responses were elicited by seeing another person with direct than averted gaze.

  • The results suggest that another individual’s direct gaze is an affiliative, positive signal.

Abstract

Facial electromyographic responses and skin conductance responses were measured to investigate whether, in a neutral laboratory environment, another individual’s direct gaze elicits a positive or negative affective reaction in the observer. The results showed that greater zygomatic responses associated with positive affect were elicited by seeing another person with direct as compared to averted gaze. The zygomatic responses were greater in response to another person’s direct gaze both when the participant’s own gaze was directed towards the other and when the participant was not looking directly towards the other. Compatible with the zygomatic responses, the corrugator activity (associated with negative affect) was decreased below baseline more in response to another person’s direct than averted gaze. Replicating previous research, the skin conductance responses were greater to another person’s direct than averted gaze. The results provide evidence that, in a neutral context, another individual’s direct gaze is an affiliative, positive signal.

Introduction

In both humans and non-human primates, eye contact can communicate messages with opposite meanings, such as friendliness or threat (Argyle and Cook, 1976, Gomez, 1996, Kleinke, 1986, Skuse, 2003, Yamagiwa, 1992). One’s interpretation of the meaning of another’s direct gaze depends on a great number of antecedent, concurrent, and anticipated contextual factors, and the outcome of this interpretation is likely to have a great influence on a perceiver’s behavioral responses. In some circumstances, direct gaze is likely to become interpreted as a positive, affiliative signal, and it is responded to, for example, with a smile and by moving closer to the gazing person, whereas in other circumstances direct gaze may evoke negative feelings leading to indifferent or even hostile behavior. An interesting question is, however, what kind of a response is elicited by another’s direct gaze in a situation which could be regarded as socially relatively neutral. Is another’s direct gaze (eye contact) inherently positive or negative? Although real-life social encounters between two persons may never be completely devoid of social contextual factors, investigation of this issue is possible in a neutral laboratory environment where many of the real-life factors influencing these responses can be controlled and eliminated.

Considering that another individual’s direct gaze is often an affiliative cue signaling the sender’s motivational tendency of approach (Adams and Kleck, 2003, Adams and Kleck, 2005), it seems likely that another’s direct gaze would elicit compatible reactions in the observer. Moreover, as humans have a fundamental need for belonging (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) and as direct gaze indicates social inclusion (Wirth, Sacco, Hugenberg, & Williams, 2010), one would expect that direct gaze would be perceived as a positive social signal evoking positive affective reactions. Compatible with these considerations, previous research has shown, for example, that seeing another person with direct as compared to averted gaze elicits increased electroencephalographic, relative left-sided frontal alpha activity associated with positive affect and motivational approach tendency (Hietanen, Leppänen, Peltola, Linna-aho, & Ruuhiala, 2008; Pönkänen, Peltola, & Hietanen, 2011). Two recent studies employing the affective priming paradigm indicated that more positive affective reactions were automatically activated by perception of direct gaze compared to perception of closed eyes (Chen, Peltola, Ranta, & Hietanen, 2016; Chen, Helminen, & Hietanen, 2017), and a study relying on the implicit association paradigm showed a robust preference for faces looking towards as compared to faces looking away (Lawson, 2015). Most recently, a study employing the startle reflex methodology reported that the magnitudes of participants’ eyeblink startle and cardiac reflexes elicited by high-intensity noise stimuli were modulated by simultaneously presented gaze direction stimuli (Chen, Peltola, Dunn, Pajunen, & Hietanen, 2017). Direct gaze attenuated the eyeblink startle and cardiac reflexes compared to those elicited in the context of a downward gaze. Thus, the defense reflexes were weaker when presented in the context of direct versus downward gaze suggesting that another’s direct gaze, compared to downward gaze, automatically elicits more positive affective responses in the viewer. In addition, studies relying on self-evaluative rating measures have shown more positive ratings to direct than averted gaze when the measurements have been conducted in “neutral” laboratory-environments (Kuzmanovic et al., 2009; Mason, Tatkow, & Macrae, 2005), although in some studies the ratings of direct gaze, even though being positive, have been less positive than those of averted gaze (Hall, Coats, & LeBeau, 2005; Hietanen et al., 2008; Pönkänen, Alhoniemi, Leppänen, & Hietanen, 2011).

In the present study, our main aim was to investigate the nature of affective reactions to another person’s direct gaze by measuring one’s facial electromyographic (EMG) responses. Measuring of facial EMG responses has been a widely used method to investigate the valence of affective reactions (Cacioppo, Petty, Losch, & Kim, 1986; Tassinary & Cacioppo, 1992). As the facial EMG responses seem to be relatively automatic, evidenced by their short latency (i.e., 300–400 ms after stimulus onset; Dimberg & Thunberg, 1998) and by their occurrence even when the stimuli are rendered invisible by backward masking (Dimberg, Thunberg, & Elmehed, 2000), this method can be seen suitable to provide more direct and objective information about one’s affective reactions than any of the methods used in the studies described above. Numerous studies have shown that affectively positive stimuli elicit increased EMG activity of the Zygomaticus major (the muscle that elevates the corners of the mouth) and decreased activity of the Corrugator supercilii (the muscle that knits the eyebrows), whereas affectively negative stimuli elicit increased activity of the Corrugator supercilii muscle (Cacioppo et al., 1986; Larsen, Norris, & Cacioppo, 2003). These EMG responses have been observed in reaction to other people’s facial (Dimberg, 1990), vocal (Hietanen, Surakka, & Linnankoski, 1998) and bodily (Magnée, Stekelenburg, Kemner, & de Gelder, 2007) expressions of emotions as well as in reaction to affective pictures of complex scenes, clips of environmental sounds, and words (Larsen et al., 2003).

There are a few previous studies measuring participants’ facial EMG responses to pictures of human faces or animated virtual characters looking towards the observer or not and expressing a facial emotion or a neutral face (Mojzisch et al., 2006; Rychlowska, Zinner, Musca, & Niedenthal, 2012; Schrammel, Pannasch, Graupner, Mojzisch, & Velichkovsky, 2009; Soussignan et al., 2013). While the results of most of these studies provided evidence that the facial reactions in response to the facial expressions were modulated by the expressor’s gaze direction (Rychlowska et al., 2012, Schrammel et al., 2009, Soussignan et al., 2013), the studies showed no effect of gaze direction on the EMG responses when there was a neutral expression on the face. However, it is possible that this was due to the fact the stimuli were images presented on a computer monitor. Images of avatars or images of real people do not look back at the viewer, not even when the gaze is directed towards the viewer. In many previous experiments from our laboratory, gaze direction has been observed to influence psychophysiological responses (electroencephalographic and autonomic responses) when participants are seeing a live person, but not when they are seeing a picture of the same person on a computer monitor (Hietanen et al., 2008; Pönkänen, Alhoniemi et al., 2011; Pönkänen, Peltola et al., 2011). Therefore, in the present study too, we investigated the effect of another’s gaze direction on facial EMG responses when the participants were facing a live person.

In everyday dyadic interactions, both interactors shift their gaze towards and away from each other. There are periods of time when neither of the partners looks towards the other partner’s eyes, periods while one of them looks at another’s eyes while the other is looking elsewhere, and periods when both look into each other’s eyes, thus, making eye contact. In the present experiment, a secondary aim was to investigate the effects of another’s gaze direction on a participant’s reactions when the participant him-/herself is looking either at the other person or slightly away. Moreover, to simulate the everyday interaction, the participants could voluntarily choose whether to look at the other individual or not. Third, this feature of the experiment allowed us also to investigate if the facial EMG responses to direct gaze could depend on whether the eye contact was voluntary or forced. It has been suggested that processing of social information is influenced by the possibility for interaction (De Jaegher, Di Paolo, & Gallagher, 2010; Schilbach et al., 2006). Supposedly, an interaction resulting from an external (experimenter’s) demand is not as rewarding as a voluntary interaction. To investigate the effect of free-choice vs. forced choice eye contact on facial EMG responses we also included in our experimental design a condition in which the participant and the model person were required to look at each other. We also measured sympathetic skin conductance responses (SCR) in order to measure, not only physiological responses related to affective valence, but also responses indexing physiological arousal (Critchley, 2002), another central component of the affective response (Plutchik, 1980). Previous studies have shown that SCRs are larger in response to seeing another’s direct gaze than averted gaze or closed eyes (Helminen, Kaasinen, & Hietanen, 2011; Hietanen et al., 2008, Myllyneva and Hietanen, 2015, Nichols and Champness, 1971).

In sum, in the present study, we measured facial EMG activity from the Zygomaticus major muscle region (cheek) and from the Corrugator supercilii muscle region (brow) and autonomic arousal (skin conductance responses, SCR) from participants when they were presented with another live person through an electronic shutter. On a majority of trials, the participants were allowed to decide whether they looked at the other person or looked sideways (at a pre-determined fixation spot). The model persons also independently varied their gaze direction between direct and averted. In addition to these free-choice trials, there were a number of trials on which the participant and the model were instructed to look at each other (i.e., forced choice trials). We expected that eye contact with another person would result in greater zygomatic EMG activity and SCRs compared to looking at another person with averted gaze. Secondly, we hypothesized that another person’s direct gaze would result in greater zygomatic EMG responses and SCRs compared to averted gaze also when the observer himself/herself is not looking directly towards the other person, but sees from the “corner of the eye” that the other is looking at him/her. This assumption was based on results from a previous study suggesting that increased psychophysiological responses to eye contact critically depend on the understanding of being watched by another (Myllyneva & Hietanen, 2015). Third, we expected that the zygomatic EMG responses and SCRs in response to eye contact would be greater if the eye contact results from participants’ free choice compared to when it is externally controlled. We also measured participants’ explicit affective feelings (affective valence and arousal) in response to different gazing conditions to compare the psychophysiological responses to these explicit evaluations.

Section snippets

Participants

The participants (N = 27) were 15 females and 12 males (age range = 22–27 years; mean age = 22.2, SD = 2.1) recruited from undergraduate psychology courses. Apart from one male, all participants were right-handed. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and they did not report of any neurological or psychiatric problems. All participants gave a written, informed consent, and received either course credits or a movie ticket for their participation. Ethical statement for the experiment

EMG responses

Zygomatic region EMG responses for the free-choice trials were analysed with a 2(Model’s gaze) × 2(Participant’s gaze) × 6(Time) ANOVA (see Fig. 1). The main effects of Model’s gaze, F(1,26) = 16.894, p = 0.0001, ηp2 = 0.394, and Participants’ gaze, F(1,26) = 6.846, p = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.208, were statistically significant. The zygomatic muscle region responses were greater in response to model’s direct gaze (M = 0.518, SEM = 0.114) than averted gaze (M = 0.016, SEM = 0.097), and also when the participant had direct gaze

Discussion

In the present study, we used measurements of facial electromyography to investigate whether another individual’s direct gaze is perceived as an affiliative, positive signal when observed in a neutral context. In this case, direct gaze was expected to result in greater zygomatic responses compared to seeing another with averted gaze. In addition to facial EMG, we also measured skin conductance responses indexing sympathetic arousal as well as self-evaluations of the affective valence and

References (55)

  • L.M. Pönkänen et al.

    The observer observed: Frontal EEG asymmetry and autonomic responses differentiate between another person’s direct and averted gaze when the face is seen live

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2011)
  • L. Schilbach et al.

    Being with virtual others: Neural correlates of social interaction

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • R.B. Adams et al.

    Perceived gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion

    Psychological Science

    (2003)
  • R.B. Adams et al.

    Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion

    Emotion

    (2005)
  • M. Argyle et al.

    Gaze and mutual gaze

    (1976)
  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human-motivation

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1995)
  • J.T. Cacioppo et al.

    Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and intensity of affective reactions

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1986)
  • T. Chen et al.

    Affective priming by eye gaze stimuli: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

    Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • T. Chen et al.

    Affect in the eyes: Explicit and implicit evaluations

    Cognition and Emotion

    (2017)
  • T. Chen et al.

    Modulation of the eyeblink and cardiac startle reflexes by genuine eye contact

    Psychophysiology

    (2017)
  • H.D. Critchley

    Electrodermal responses: What happens in the brain

    Neuroscientist

    (2002)
  • M.E. Dawson et al.

    The electrodermal system

  • U. Dimberg et al.

    Rapid facial reactions to emotional facial expressions

    Scandinavian Journal of Psychology

    (1998)
  • U. Dimberg et al.

    Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions

    Psychological Science

    (2000)
  • U. Dimberg

    Facial reactions to facial expressions

    Psychophysiology

    (1982)
  • U. Dimberg

    Facial electromyography and emotional reactions

    Psychophysiology

    (1990)
  • P. Ekman et al.

    The Duchenne smile: Emotional expression and brain physiology II

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1990)
  • Cited by (23)

    • Being watched by a humanoid robot and a human: Effects on affect-related psychophysiological responses

      2022, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      This finding would indicate that direct gaze induces these responses only when it is accompanied by the experience of being seen by the robot/human model. For the facial EMG responses, however, previous studies have indicated that the facial reactions may not be sensitive to this type of top-down influence (Hietanen et al., 2018, 2019, 2020). Thus, for the EMG responses, we expected the effect of gaze direction regardless of BW vs. BnW condition.

    • Making eye contact with a robot: Psychophysiological responses to eye contact with a human and with a humanoid robot

      2021, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, the subjective evaluations of affective valence match with the results of the physiological measurements within each block. It should be noted that some of the previous studies conducted with a live human model have reported less positive feelings to another’s direct than averted gaze (or no difference between the gaze directions) (Hietanen et al., 2008, 2018; Pönkänen, Alhoniemi et al., 2011). This pattern of results has been suggested to be associated with the feelings of uneasiness evoked by being a target of someone’s attention (Pönkänen, Alhoniemi et al., 2011).

    • Eye contact in virtual reality – A psychophysiological study

      2020, Computers in Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      This exceeds the required sample size for finding a medium effect (d = 0.50) at 0.80 power and α level of 0.05 (Cohen, 1992; Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007). Because the effect of live direct vs. averted gaze on SCRs is large (reported ds ranging from 0.70 to 0.98; Hietanen et al., 2018; Myllyneva & Hietanen, 2015b), we thus expected statistical power to be sufficient for detecting the effect even if it was diminished in the VR condition (i.e., a medium effect of d = 0.50). Participants were required to be at least 15 years old and to not have diagnosed psychiatric or neurological disorders.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This research was supported by the Finnish Cultural Foundation and the Academy of Finland grant #266187 (MIND program) to J.K.H. and the Academy of Finland grant #275519 to M.J.P.

    View full text