Elsevier

Biological Psychology

Volume 117, May 2016, Pages 131-140
Biological Psychology

Deficient aversive-potentiated startle and the triarchic model of psychopathy: The role of boldness

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.03.012Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Boldness alone is related to deficient aversive-potentiated startle.

  • Boldness rather than meanness better reflects the low-fear disposition of psychopathy.

  • The boldness-related fear deficit is specific for threat, but not mutilation, scenes.

  • Triarchic model of psychopathy is a powerful venue to examine deficits in psychopathy.

Abstract

This study examined the contribution of the phenotypic domains of boldness, meanness, and disinhibition of the triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy (Patrick, Fowles, & Krueger, 2009) to deficient aversive-potentiated startle in a mixed-gender sample of 180 undergraduates. Eyeblink responses to noise probes were recorded during a passive picture-viewing task (erotica, neutral, threat, and mutilation). Deficient threat vs. neutral potentiation was uniquely related to increased boldness scores, thus suggesting that the diminished defensive reaction to aversive stimulation is specifically linked to the charm, social potency and venturesomeness features of psychopathy (boldness), but not to features such as callousness, coldheartedness and cruelty traits (meanness), even though both phenotypes theoretically share the same underlying low-fear disposition. Our findings provide further evidence of the differential association between distinct psychopathy components and deficits in defensive reactivity and strongly support the validity of the triarchic model of psychopathy in disentangling the etiology of this personality disorder.

Introduction

Psychopathy is a multifaceted personality disorder that is characterized by a cluster of interpersonal, affective, lifestyle and antisocial traits and behaviors, including deception, manipulation, irresponsibility, poor behavioral control, shallow affect, a lack of empathy, guilt or remorse, and a range of unethical and antisocial behaviors, that are not necessarily criminal (Hare, 2007). Although multiple psychological theories have attempted to explain the psychopathy construct, controversy about its definition and nature remains (cf. Skeem & Cooke, 2010).

For a long time, the dominant theoretical perspective on psychopathy has conceptualized this personality disorder as a unitary syndrome that arises from a core underlying pathology or deficit. One of the more influential and supported etiological theories included in this unitary-syndrome perspective is the low fear hypothesis, which proposes that psychopaths display a deficit in emotional reactivity that specifically relates to neurobiological systems that modulate fear—that is, psychopathic individuals may be marked by an under-reactivity of the brain’s aversive/defensive motivational system (Lykken, 1995). The low fear hypothesis of psychopathy has been supported by different psychophysiological correlates and diverse experimental procedures. One of the most reliable indicators of fear reactivity deficits in psychopathy is a blunted startle reflex potentiation (Patrick, 1994). Startle reflex is an automatic defensive reaction to a sudden, intense event (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1990). Research in normal individuals has widely demonstrated that the magnitude of the startle blink response is modulated by the affective valence of the stimulus context in which it is evoked (Lang et al., 1990); the startle blink response is normally attenuated during exposure to appetitive contexts (startle inhibition), and it is increased under aversive conditions (startle potentiation). Consistent with the unitary view of psychopathy, multiple studies have demonstrated that incarcerated psychopaths do not show the typical startle potentiation during aversive stimulation processing (Levenston, Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, 2000; Pastor, Moltó, Vila, & Lang, 2003; Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, 1993).

Later, dual-process models of psychopathy emerged (Fowles and Dindo, 2006, Fowles and Dindo, 2009, Patrick and Bernat, 2009), challenging the unitary view of psychopathy. The dual- or two-process conceptualization posits that separate neural mechanisms differentially contribute to the affective-interpersonal and impulsive-antisocial components of psychopathy, namely, trait fearlessness, which reflects a deficit or under-reactivity of the brain’s aversive/defensive motivational system, and externalizing vulnerability, which reflects impairments in the frontocortical systems that mediate anticipation, planning, and inhibitory control (for empirical evidence, see, for example, Carlson, Thái, & McLarnon, 2009; Heritage & Benning, 2013; López, Poy, Patrick, & Moltó, 2013; Moltó, Poy, Segarra, Pastor, & Montañés, 2007; Patrick, Durbin, & Moser, 2012; Patrick & Lang, 1999; Venables, Hall, Yancey, & Patrick, 2015). Consistent with this perspective, studies examining the differential contribution of psychopathy components to reduced startle potentiation have demonstrated that this deficit is specifically related to the interpersonal and affective features of psychopathy, but not to its externalizing features. More importantly, this association has been confirmed in different samples, even when assessing affective-interpersonal traits via different psychopathy measures. In this regard, the deficient startle potentiation in psychopaths has been related to Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 2003) Factor 1, which encompasses the interpersonal and affective features of psychopathy, in incarcerated men (Patrick, 1994; Vaidyanathan, Hall, Patrick, & Bernat, 2011) and women (Verona, Bresin, & Patrick, 2013), as well as in mixed-gender community populations (Vanman, Mejia, Dawson, Schell, & Raine, 2003). Additionally, this deficit has been associated with the Fearless Dominance factor of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory-Revised (PPI-R; Lilienfeld & Widows, 2005) in community men (Benning, Patrick, & Iacono, 2005; estimated from Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire; Tellegen & Waller, 2008) and women (Anderson, Stanford, Wan, & Young, 2011), as well as in mixed-gender community populations (Poy et al., 2012). Therefore, research results suggest that the startle potentiation deficit (as a valid indicator of deficient fear reactivity) is specifically linked to the core affective and interpersonal features of psychopathy, irrespective of the sample characteristics (criminal, non-criminal) and gender. Furthermore, this finding suggests that only the charm, fearlessness, emotional detachment and low anxiety traits of psychopathy reflect an underlying weakness in the brain’s core aversive/defensive motivational system.

It is remarkable that defensive deficits in psychopathy have been associated with indicators of emotional and interpersonal traits from different psychopathy measures (PCL-R Factor 1 and PPI-R Fearless Dominance), even though they do not seem to assess the affective/interpersonal features of the disorder in the same manner. First, PCL-R Factor 1 is described by selfishness, callousness, and the remorseless use of others (Hare, 1991, Hare, 2003), whereas the PPI-R Fearless Dominance is defined by low trait anxiousness, social dominance, and fearless risk taking (Benning, Patrick, Hicks, Blonigen, & Krueger, 2003). Second, PCL-R Factor 1 and PPI-R Fearless Dominance show small- to medium-sized correlations (Baskin-Sommers, Zeier, & Newman, 2009; Benning, Patrick, Blonigen, Hicks, & Iacono, 2005; Berardino, Meloy, Sherman, & Jacobs, 2005; Malterer, Lilienfeld, Neumann, & Newman, 2010), and although related, PCL-R Factor 1 and PPI-R Fearless Dominance only share a small amount of variance (4%; Marcus, Fulton, & Edens, 2013). Last, PCL-R Factor 1 does not measure anxiety and fear directly in any of its items (Hare, 2003) and is weakly and inconsistently correlated with anxiety and anxiety-related scales (Hare, 1991, Hare, 2003; Sandvik, Hansen, Hystad, Johnsen, & Bartone, 2015; Schmitt & Newman, 1999; Vitale, Smith, Brinkley, & Newman, 2002; Weizmann-Henelius, Viemerö, & Eronen, 2004). On the other hand, PPI-R Fearless Dominance directly assesses the traits of fearlessness and stress immunity, which are closer to classic descriptions of primary psychopathy than PCL-R (cf. Marcus et al., 2013). In contrast to PCL-R Factor 1, PPI-R Fearless Dominance shows negative associations with anxiety indices and questionnaires (Benning et al., 2003, Benning et al., 2005, Edens and McDermott, 2010, Lilienfeld and Widows, 2005; Patrick, Edens, Poythress, Lilienfeld, & Benning, 2006; Ross et al., 2007; Uzieblo, Verschuere, & Crombez, 2007; Uzieblo, Verschuere, Van den Bussche, & Crombez, 2010).

Hence, although both PCL-R Factor 1 and PPI-R Fearless Dominance evaluate the affective and interpersonal traits of psychopathy, it seems that these instruments assess different configurations of interpersonal-affective characteristics. This predicament brings into question whether both clusters of affective-interpersonal traits – PCL-R Factor 1 and PPI-R Fearless Dominance – are equally related to the low fear temperament of psychopathy or, by contrast, one of them might be particularly relevant over and above the other to understand defensive reactivity deficits in psychopathy. Likewise, it is also possible that the contribution of each particular affective-interpersonal cluster to psychopathy-related deficits varies depending on the characteristics of the sample. For example, some affective-interpersonal traits might be more relevant to explain psychopaths’ fear deficit in successful individuals (psychopaths that refrain from serious antisocial behavior), but not in unsuccessful individuals (and vice versa). Thus, examining the contribution of simpler configurations of affective-interpersonal psychopathy traits – instead of clustering them in a single component – to psychopathy-related deficits may help to clarify the specific psychopathy features that are particularly related to trait fearlessness.

The recently proposed triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy (Patrick, Fowles, & Krueger, 2009) could be useful to disentangle whether the above mentioned defensive deficit is related to both clusters of psychopathic personality features or, by contrast, whether it is specifically linked to one of them. Thus, this model classifies the affective and interpersonal traits of psychopathy into two distinct phenotypes, namely, boldness and meanness (and adds a third phenotype, disinhibition, which is related to the externalizing tendencies of the disorder). These three constructs have distinctive phenotypic identities and can be conceptualized, measured, and understood separately (although they are interrelated at some levels empirically, as well as in terms of their mutual connections with the phenomenon of psychopathy; Patrick et al., 2009). The triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy describes disinhibition as a general trend towards impulse control problems, including a lack of planning and foresight, impaired regulation of affect and impulses, an insistence on behaviors that involve immediate gratification, and a deficient control of behavior. In turn, boldness encompasses a propensity to remain calm in situations involving pressure or threat, the ability to easily recover from stressful events, high self-confidence, social effectiveness, and tolerance for unfamiliarity or dangers. Overall, this construct reflects the Cleckley traditional descriptions of psychopathy of social efficacy, the apparent absence of anxiety or neurotic psychopathology, a diminished affective responsiveness and certain punishment immunity (Cleckley, 1976Cleckley, 1941/Cleckley, 19761976). Finally, meanness describes a set of attributes including low empathy, indifference and lack of attachment relationships, rebelliousness, sensation seeking, tendency to exploit others, and cruelty. In contrast to boldness, which emphasizes the description of non-criminal psychopathy, meanness is related to descriptions of criminal psychopaths (cf. McCord & McCord, 1964).

Etiologically, the triarchic model suggests that boldness and meanness are distinct phenotypic manifestations of the psychopaths’ trait fearlessness; that is, both constructs share a fearlessness genotype as an etiological substrate (Patrick et al., 2009) that evolves into a boldness or meanness phenotype depending on certain developmental factors (such as a difficult temperament, or a failure of secure attachment). Then, the triarchic model provides a novel conceptualization of psychopathy that considers the affective-interpersonal component of psychopathy in terms of more elemental constructs or clusters. On the one hand, boldness encompasses the charm, persuasiveness, imperturbability, and venturesomeness characteristics of psychopathy (which would be directly measured by PPI-R Fearless Dominance; cf. Patrick et al., 2009), and on the other hand, meanness encompasses the deficient empathy, lack of close attachments, rebelliousness, and exploitativeness characteristics of the disorder (traits emphasized in PCL-R Factor 1; cf. Patrick et al., 2009). Thus, the triarchic perspective offers a valuable framework that can clarify the core traits that are directly related to the manifestation of fearlessness-related deficits in psychopaths. In this regard, examining aversive-potentiated startle deficits from a triarchic view may help to elucidate the differential contribution of affective-interpersonal clusters of psychopathy traits (represented differentially in boldness and meanness) to the manifestation of the psychopaths’ trait fearlessness in different populations.

Hence, although past research has successfully associated a blunted startle potentiation with the affective-interpersonal component of psychopathy (Anderson et al., 2011, Benning et al., 2005, Patrick et al., 1993, Vaidyanathan et al., 2011, Vanman et al., 2003, Verona et al., 2013), recent proposals suggest that this component could encompass a wide variety of traits that are distributed into different clusters of affective and interpersonal characteristics—which the triarchic conceptualization (Patrick et al., 2009) could aid to disentangle. To provide new insights into the specific affective and interpersonal traits of psychopathy that are related to deficits in defensive (fear) reactivity in non-incarcerated participants, here we explored the differential contribution of boldness, meanness and disinhibition to the affective modulation of the startle reflex during an affective picture-viewing task in a mixed-gender undergraduate sample. Based on the theoretical description of the boldness and meanness domains – which suggests that both boldness and meanness are phenotypical expressions of a fearlessness genotype (Patrick et al., 2009) – as well as previous findings for psychopathy components in relation to deficient startle potentiation, here we hypothesized that this deficit would be related to boldness and/or meanness, but not to the disinhibition domain.

A secondary aim of this study was to evaluate whether psychopathy-related deficits in startle modulation depend on the specific aversive content depicted, an issue that has been barely addressed in the previous literature. Studies on the psychopaths’ blunted startle potentiation during aversive stimulation have usually collapsed different picture contents – such as mutilations, victimization scenes, aimed guns or attacking animals – into one broad unpleasant category (cf. Benning, Patrick et al., 2005, Pastor et al., 2003, Patrick et al., 1993, Vanman et al., 2003). The few studies that have specifically examined the psychopaths’ startle modulation for discrete aversive picture contents have yielded inconsistent results. For example, incarcerated psychopaths in Levenston et al. (2000) study showed an abnormal startle inhibition (instead of potentiation) for victim scenes (mutilated figures or attacks on others) and non-significant enhanced startle reactions for direct threat. In contrast, Vaidyanathan et al. (2011) reported a significant deficit in startle potentiation for threatening contents, but not for mutilations in high PCL-R Factor 1 prisoners. In the same vein, Vaidyanathan et al. (2009) found that undergraduates high in fearlessness displayed the expected pattern of startle potentiation for mutilation and victim scenes, but failed to potentiate for the pictures depicting threat. These results might tentatively suggest content-specific associations between a deficient aversive-potentiated startle and the affective and interpersonal traits of psychopathy. Therefore, we further explored, in a subsample of the study, psychopathy-related differences in startle potentiation for two distinct picture contents within the unpleasant category—threat and mutilations.

Section snippets

Participants

A large sample of 180 undergraduates (72 men, 108 women) from the Universitat Jaume I of Castellón (Spain), aged between 17 and 43 years (M = 20.62, SD = 4.01), participated for course credit or 10€ as compensation. No participant was undergoing psychiatric or pharmacological treatment at the time of testing, and none presented non-corrected visual or auditory deficits. All participants were informed about the nature of the study and provided informed consent.

The Spanish adaptation (Poy, Segarra,

Construct validity of the TriPM

Pearson bivariate and partial correlations of the TriPM scale scores with criterion variables consisting of personality trait measures in the current sample are presented in Table 2.2 Boldness scores were strongly positively

Discussion

The primary aim of the present study was to examine the contribution of the triarchic psychopathy domains to the under-reactivity of the aversive/defensive motivational system – as indexed by deficient startle potentiation (cf. Patrick, 1994) – in a mixed-gender community sample assessed for psychopathy. Specifically, here we investigated the differential relationships between the two distinct clusters of primary psychopathy traits that the triarchic conceptualization embodies in the boldness

Acknowledgments

This study received the support of Grant PSI2011-22559 from the Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (Spain), Grant P1·1B2013-12 from Universitat Jaume I (Spain) and a predoctoral fellowship from the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (Spain) to the first author (BES-2008-004554).

References (88)

  • P.R. Almeida et al.

    Empathic, moral and antisocial outcomes associated with distinct components of psychopathy in healthy individuals: a Triarchic model approach

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2015)
  • N.E. Anderson et al.

    High psychopathic trait females exhibit reduced startle potentiation and increased P3 amplitude

    Behavioral Sciences and the Law

    (2011)
  • M. Balaban et al.

    Off-line latency and amplitude scoring of the human reflex eyeblink with Fortran IV

    Psychophysiology

    (1986)
  • A.R. Baskin-Sommers et al.

    Self-reported attentional control differentiates the major factors of psychopathy

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2009)
  • S.D. Benning et al.

    Estimating facets of psychopathy from normal personality traits: a step toward community epidemiological investigations

    Assessment

    (2005)
  • S.D. Benning et al.

    Factor structure of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory: validity and implications for clinical assessment

    Psychological Assessment

    (2003)
  • S.D. Benning et al.

    Psychopathy, startle blink modulation, and electrodermal reactivity in twin men

    Psychophysiology

    (2005)
  • S.D. Berardino et al.

    Validation of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory on a female inmate sample

    Behavioral Sciences and the Law

    (2005)
  • T.D. Blumenthal et al.

    Committee report: guidelines for human startle eyeblink electromyographic studies

    Psychophysiology

    (2005)
  • M.M. Bradley et al.

    Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing

    Emotion

    (2001)
  • S.J. Brislin et al.

    Development and validation of triarchic psychopathy scales from the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire

    Psychological Assessment

    (2015)
  • S.R. Carlson et al.

    Visual P3 amplitude and self-reported psychopathic personality traits: frontal reduction is associated with Self-Centered Impulsivity

    Psychophysiology

    (2009)
  • H. Cleckley

    The mask of sanity

    (1941/1976)
  • E.W. Cook

    VPM reference manual

    (2001)
  • P.T. Costa et al.

    Inventario de personalidad NEO revisado (NEO-PI-R) e inventario NEO reducido de cinco factores (NEO-FFI). Manual profesional

    (2002)
  • B.N. Cuthbert et al.

    Probing picture perception: activation and emotion

    Psychophysiology

    (1996)
  • L. Dindo et al.

    Dual temperamental risk factors for psychopathic personality: evidence from self-report and skin conductance

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2011)
  • J.J. Donahue et al.

    Examining the triarchic model of psychopathy using revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2015)
  • L.E. Drislane et al.

    A triarchic model analysis of the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory

    Journal of Personality Disorders

    (2015)
  • L.E. Drislane et al.

    Clarifying the content coverage of differing psychopathy inventories through reference to the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure

    Psychological Assessment

    (2014)
  • J.F. Edens et al.

    Examining the construct validity of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory-Revised: preferential correlates of fearless dominance and self-centered impulsivity

    Psychological Assessment

    (2010)
  • D.C. Fowles et al.

    A dual-deficit model of psychopathy

  • D.C. Fowles et al.

    Temperament and psychopathy: a dual-pathway model

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (2009)
  • J.R. Hall et al.

    Development and validation of triarchic construct scales from the Psychopathic Personality Inventory

    Psychological Assessment

    (2014)
  • R.D. Hare

    The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised

    (1991)
  • R.D. Hare

    The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised

    (2003)
  • R.D. Hare

    Psychological instruments in the assessment of psychopathy

  • A.J. Heritage et al.

    Impulsivity and response modulation deficits in psychopathy: evidence from ERN and N1

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2013)
  • J.R. Jennings

    Editorial policy on analyses of variance with repeated measures

    Psychophysiology

    (1987)
  • R.F. Krueger et al.

    Linking antisocial behavior, substance abuse, and personality: an integrative quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2007)
  • R. López et al.

    Deficient fear conditioning and self-reported psychopathy: the role of fearless dominance

    Psychophysiology

    (2013)
  • P.J. Lang et al.

    International Affective Picture System (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8

    (2008)
  • P.J. Lang et al.

    Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex

    Psychological Review

    (1990)
  • G.K. Levenston et al.

    The psychopath as observer: emotion and attention in picture processing

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2000)
  • S.O. Lilienfeld et al.

    The self-report assessment of psychopathy: problems, pitfalls, and promises

  • S.O. Lilienfeld et al.

    The role of fearless dominance in psychopathy: confusions, controversies, and clarifications

    Personality Disorders: Theory Research, and Treatment

    (2012)
  • S.O. Lilienfeld et al.

    Psychopathic Personality Inventory-Revised (PPI-R) professional manual

    (2005)
  • S. Lissek et al.

    The strong situation: a potential impediment to studying the psychobiology and pharmacology of anxiety disorders

    Biological Psychology

    (2006)
  • D.T. Lykken

    The antisocial personalities

    (1995)
  • M.B. Malterer et al.

    Concurrent validity of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory with offender and community samples

    Assessment

    (2010)
  • D.K. Marcus et al.

    The two-factor model of psychopathic personality: evidence from the Psychopathic Personality Inventory

    Personality Disorders: Theory Research and Treatment

    (2013)
  • D.K. Marcus et al.

    A new measure of attitudes toward sexually predatory tactics and its relation to the triarchic model of psychopathy

    Journal of Personality Disorders

    (2014)
  • B.E. Marion et al.

    An examination of the association between psychopathy and dissimulation using the MMPI-2-RF validity scales

    Law and Human Behavior

    (2013)
  • W. McCord et al.

    The psychopath: an essay on the criminal mind

    (1964)
  • Cited by (41)

    • The implicit measurement of psychopathy

      2023, Journal of Research in Personality
    • Trait boldness and emotion regulation: An event-related potential investigation

      2022, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      This notion is supported by research demonstrating negative associations between boldness and reactivity to phasic aversive stimuli. Specifically, in the context of viewing unpleasant versus neutral pictures, community participants higher in boldness show smaller potentiation of the startle response to sudden auditory probes (i.e., reduced fear-potentiated startle; Benning et al., 2005b; Esteller et al., 2016). In addition, incarcerated individuals diagnosed with psychopathy, which includes elements of boldness (Venables et al., 2014), also exhibit reduced aversive startle potentiation (Patrick, 1994; Vaidyanathan et al., 2011).

    • The startle reflex as an indicator of psychopathic personality from childhood to adulthood: A systematic review

      2021, Acta Psychologica
      Citation Excerpt :

      Three other studies also reported reduced ASP in relation to PPI-R fearless-dominance (Anderson & Stanford, 2012; Justus & Finn, 2007; Vaidyanathan, Patrick, & Bernat, 2009). Work by Esteller et al. (2016) also lends support for prior work on fearless-dominance and ASP; individuals high in boldness (as measured with Triarchic Psychopahy Measure [TriPM]) demonstrated reduced ASP, but similar relations were not found for those high in meanness or disinhibition. Further, similar findings were reported by Vanman et al. (2003), who demonstrated that individuals high on Factor 1-scores (affective-interpersonal) of the PCL-R in a sample of unemployed males seeking temporary work showed reduced ASP.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text