Elsevier

Behavior Therapy

Volume 44, Issue 3, September 2013, Pages 470-478
Behavior Therapy

Reactivity to Exclusion Prospectively Predicts Social Anxiety Symptoms in Young Adults

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2013.04.007Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We tested the effects of peer victimization on social anxiety over time

  • We utilized Cyberball computer task to simulate peer victimization

  • Reactivity to exclusion prospectively predicted social anxiety symptoms

  • Self-reported past and current teasing did not predict social anxiety

  • Interventions could target reactivity to peer victimization

Abstract

Peer victimization leads to negative outcomes such as increased anxiety and depression. The prospective relationship between peer victimization and social anxiety in children and adolescents is well established, and adults with social anxiety disorder (SAD) are more likely than individuals with other anxiety disorders to report a history of teasing. However, a crucial bridge between these findings (peer victimization in young adults) is missing. We manipulated perceptions of peer exclusion in a young adult sample (N = 108) using the Cyberball Ostracism Task. Reactivity to exclusion prospectively predicted social anxiety symptoms at a 2-month follow-up, whereas self-reported teasing during high school and current relational victimization did not. This research suggests that reactions to peer victimization may be a worthwhile target for clinical interventions in young adults. Targeting how young adults react to stressful social interactions such as exclusion may help prevent the development of SAD. Future research should test if reactivity to exclusion plays a role in the relationship between other disorders (e.g., depression) and peer victimization.

Section snippets

Participants

A total of 108 participants completed the Time 1 session and 100 participants returned to complete the Time 2 session to receive credit as part of their coursework at a midwestern metropolitan university. Participants were mostly white (n = 63; 58.3%) women (n = 67; 62.0%). Other ethnicities reported were Asian/Pacific Islander (n = 34; 31.5%), Black (n = 6; 5.6%), multiracial (n = 4; 3.7%), and Hispanic (n = 1; .9%). Two thirds of our participants were in either their freshman or sophomore year of college

Results

Table 1 displays means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations among all variables. In a single model, all Time 1 predictors were used to predict all Time 2 constructs. The initial model fit was perfect by definition because the model was saturated. However, model fit when nonsignificant paths (ps > .10) were dropped was excellent (df = 6; CFI = 1.00, TLI = 1.00, RMSEA = .00, SRMR = .03). As can be seen in Figure 1, reactivity to exclusion (estimate = .16, p = .008) was the only significant predictor of

Discussion

We found that reactivity to exclusion simulated by the Cyberball computer task prospectively predicted social anxiety 2 months later. Against hypothesis, and in contrast with findings regarding children and adolescents, past teasing through high school and current relational victimization at Time 1 did not prospectively predict social anxiety. These results bridge findings between childhood and adolescent research and research on adults with SAD (e.g., Erath et al., 2008, McCabe et al., 2003,

References (35)

  • N.I. Eisenberger et al.

    Functional magnetic resonance imaging responses relate to differences in real-world social experience

    Emotion

    (2007)
  • N.I. Eisenberger et al.

    Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion

    Science

    (2003)
  • S.A. Erath et al.

    Early adolescent school adjustment: Associations with friendship and peer victimization

    Social Development

    (2008)
  • P. Gilbert

    Depression: The evolution of powerlessness

    (1992)
  • D.J. Hawker et al.

    Twenty years’ research on peer victimization and psychosocial maladjustment: A meta-analytic review of cross-sectional studies

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • R.G. Heimberg et al.

    A cognitive-behavioral model of social anxiety disorder: Update and extension

  • R.G. Heimberg et al.

    Assessment of social phobia

  • Cited by (35)

    • Social anxiety moderates the relationship between body mass index and motivation to avoid exercise

      2021, Body Image
      Citation Excerpt :

      We created a composite score because composite measures generally provide more reliable estimates of constructs (Zeller & Carmines, 1980) and reduce the number of analyses conducted. Similar composite measures have been used in previous studies of SA (Clark et al., 2003, 2006; Kaplan, Levinson, Rodebaugh, Menatti, & Weeks, 2015; Levinson, Langer, & Rodebaugh, 2013) and have demonstrated very good internal consistency (e.g., Kaplan et al., 2015) and test-retest reliability (Levinson et al., 2013). To create the composite SA score, we summed the standardized scores of every item from each of the two measures.

    • Perceived burdensomeness and neural responses to ostracism in the Cyberball task

      2020, Journal of Psychiatric Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The current sample showed average scores of 10.1 ± 6.1 and 26.2 ± 13.7 for PB and TB, respectively. As social anxiety and depression can impact both the performance and brain responses during the Cyberball task (Heeren et al., 2017; Jobst et al., 2015; Levinson et al., 2013), we assessed these characteristics using the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale (SIAS) (Mattick and Clarke, 1998) and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1996). The 20-item SIAS evaluates fear of general social interaction, with each item scored from 0 to 4 (3 items are reverse coded) and higher scores indicating greater anxiety.

    • Can words be worse than stones? Understanding distressing social events and their relationship with social anxiety

      2020, Journal of Anxiety Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      First, frequencies of both antagonistic and non-antagonistic distressing social events were expected to be related to social anxiety (e.g., Storch & Masia-Warner, 2004). Second, responses to distressing social events captured by a measure of PTSD symptoms were expected to have a stronger relationship with social anxiety than the frequencies of the different kinds of antagonistic and non-antagonistic distressing social events (Carleton et al., 2011; Levinson et al., 2013; Rosen et al., 2012). Third, responses to distressing social events captured by a measure of PTSD symptoms were expected to statistically mediate the relationships between distressing social event frequencies and social anxiety.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    We would like to thank Simona C. Kaplan and Lauren Kramer for their research assistance. This research was funded in part by F31-MH096433-01 to Cheri A. Levinson.

    View full text