Elsevier

Brain and Cognition

Volume 123, June 2018, Pages 110-119
Brain and Cognition

Early enhanced processing and delayed habituation to deviance sounds in autism spectrum disorder

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2018.03.004Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Children with ASD exhibit increased ERP responses to auditory changes.

  • Temporal dynamics indicate earlier and prolonged detection of auditory deviants.

  • Individual differences are related to sensory-seeking problems in ASD.

Abstract

Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) exhibit difficulties processing and encoding sensory information in daily life. Cognitive response to environmental change in control individuals is naturally dynamic, meaning it habituates or reduces over time as one becomes accustomed to the deviance. The origin of atypical response to deviance in ASD may relate to differences in this dynamic habituation. The current study of 133 children and young adults with and without ASD examined classic electrophysiological responses (MMN and P3a), as well as temporal patterns of habituation (i.e., N1 and P3a change over time) in response to a passive auditory oddball task. Individuals with ASD showed an overall heightened sensitivity to change as exhibited by greater P3a amplitude to novel sounds. Moreover, youth with ASD showed dynamic ERP differences, including slower attenuation of the N1 response to infrequent tones and the P3a response to novel sounds. Dynamic ERP responses were related to parent ratings of auditory sensory-seeking behaviors, but not general cognition. As the first large-scale study to characterize temporal dynamics of auditory ERPs in ASD, our results provide compelling evidence that heightened response to auditory deviance in ASD is largely driven by early sensitivity and prolonged processing of auditory deviance.

Introduction

Detection of change in the environment is a fundamental element of human perception. Pre-attentive neural encoding of change mediates the automatic evaluation of incoming information and reflexively triggers attention to important information in the environment (Schröger, 1997). As such, disruption to mechanisms responsible for low-level processing of change may negatively affect attention orienting and environmental adaptation. Evidence suggests that these key elements of sensation and perception are disrupted among individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by social communication impairments and restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests, and may contribute to common sensory-related features of the disorder (e.g., hypersensitivity, hyposensitivity, sensory-seeking behaviors) (Lane, Young, Baker, & Angley, 2010). Although auditory hypersensitivity is one of the most commonly reported sensory processing impairments in ASD (Gomes et al., 2004, Tomchek and Dunn, 2007), there is evidence that it co-occurs with increased sensory seeking (Liss, Saulnier, Fein, & Kinsbourne, 2006).

Atypical electrophysiological responses to stimulus change at early stages of sensory information processing have been reported in ASD and associated with behavioral symptoms, such as sensory and attention problems (Cui et al., 2016, Keehn et al., 2013, Orekhova and Stroganova, 2014). The auditory oddball task (Polich, 2007) is commonly used to assess initial detection and discrimination of an infrequent (deviant) auditory tone relative to a frequent tone by measuring an electrophysiological difference component known as the mismatch negativity (MMN) (Näätänen, Paavilainen, Rinne, & Alho, 2007). The MMN is measured and computed as the difference between electrophysiological responses to frequent and deviant stimuli across frontocentral electrodes approximately 100–200 ms post-stimulus onset, overlapping and in part driven by the N1 subcomponent (Jääskeläinen et al., 2004). If a stimulus is sufficiently deviant, such as an unexpected novel stimulus, the MMN is followed by a P3, a frontocentral positive-going ERP component occurring approximately 250–350 ms post stimulus that represents early attention orienting (Friedman et al., 2001, Polich, 2007).

Over the past three decades, studies of children and adults with ASD have reported atypical MMN/N1 and P3/P3a/P3b responses to the auditory oddball task (Cléry et al., 2013, Čeponienė et al., 2003, Ferri et al., 2003, Gomot et al., 2011, Gomot et al., 2002, Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003, Kemner et al., 1999, Kemner et al., 1995, Korpilahti et al., 2007, Kujala et al., 2007, Kujala et al., 2005, Lepistö et al., 2007, Lepistö et al., 2006, Yu et al., 2015). However, discrepant findings in the literature regarding the timing and magnitude of these responses in ASD have led to different conclusions about how stages of neural processing diverge from typical development and how dysfunctional auditory deviance detection relates to behavioral impairments. All three potential patterns of results have been identified: (1) ASD group differences suggesting a reduced or hyposensitive ASD response (i.e., smaller amplitudes, slower latencies) for the MMN (M. A. Dunn et al., 2008, Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003, Kujala et al., 2005, Kujala et al., 2007, Lepistö et al., 2006, Seri et al., 1999) and P3/P3a/P3b (Courchesne et al., 1984, Čeponienė et al., 2003, Ferri et al., 2003, Kemner et al., 1995, Lepistö et al., 2006, Lepistö et al., 2007, Lepistö et al., 2005); (2) ASD group differences suggesting enhanced or hypersensitive response (i.e., larger amplitudes, faster latencies) for the MMN (Ferri et al., 2003, Korpilahti et al., 2007, Lepistö et al., 2006) and P3/P3a/P3b (Ferri et al., 2003, Gomot et al., 2002, Gomot et al., 2011, Kujala et al., 2007, Lepistö et al., 2007); and (3) no group difference between ASD and controls, suggesting intact auditory processing (Dunn et al., 2008, Kemner et al., 1995). These distinct patterns of results in ASD may reflect methodological decisions, including stimulus types (i.e., kind of deviance) and task demands (i.e., passive or active task), but may also be driven by individual differences (e.g., cognitive abilities, development/maturation).

Critically, an additional gap in the existing research on auditory deviance detection in ASD is the contribution of dynamic processing across time, specifically habituation or sensitization of neural responses over the course of the experiment. Discrepancies in the literature may be explained by atypicalities in this dynamic pattern, with group differences potentially amplified, attenuated, or washed out entirely depending on the length of the experimental procedure, the type of deviant stimuli, and the number of ERP trials. Thus, the current study aims to expand on existing research by characterizing the temporal dynamics of electrophysiological response to two types of auditory deviance in ASD. The detection of environmental change requires complex integration of stimulus perception and encoding within memory and cognitive systems (Johnson, Spencer, Luck, & Schöner, 2009) and this process evolves over the course of an experiment (Rockstroh et al., 2011). Evidence of slower neural activation in ASD (Kleinhans et al., 2009, Kleinhans et al., 2015) suggests important differences may be tethered to dynamic patterns rather than an overall capacity for deviance detection. For instance, infants at risk for developing ASD exhibited a lack of habituation (i.e., they did not show decrement of the signal) during an auditory oddball task (Guiraud et al., 2011), consistent with accounts of atypical sensory processing. Work with school-age children with ASD similarly demonstrated a lack of habituation (Hudac et al., 2015) or atypically delayed responses due to sensitization (i.e., increase of signal) (Hudac et al., 2017) in social perception tasks.

In this study, we aimed to clarify the dynamic nature of auditory deviance detection in a large, carefully characterized sample of youth with ASD (n = 102) and without (n = 31) using a passive, auditory oddball task during EEG acquisition. We targeted the temporally dynamic aspect of discrimination between repeatedly presented “frequent” tones and (a) rare ”infrequent” tones (frequency deviance) or (b) unique, non-repeated “novel” sounds (novelty deviance) by measuring change in ERP response to these deviant sounds over time. To this end, we examined polynomial (i.e., linear, quadratic) effects of time on the N1 (frequency deviance) and P3a (novelty deviance) over the course of the experiment, as these ERP components are primary contributors to the averaged MMN and P3a signals. We anticipated atypical dynamic patterns in ASD would indicate a lack of habituation (Guiraud et al., 2011, Hudac et al., 2015). Lastly, considering known associations between behaviors and sensory sensitivities (Boyd et al., 2010, Boyd et al., 2009), we examined how individual ERP patterns were associated with sensory-related behaviors in ASD.

Section snippets

Participants

142 children and young adults age 4–23 years with ASD (N = 108; 22 female) and typically developing control children (N = 34; 11 female) were enrolled in this study and completed the EEG session. The final sample of 133 individuals comprises individuals who provided clean EEG data and characterization details are provided in Table 1. Parents gave written consent and participants provided written consent or written assent as chronological- and mental-age appropriate. The local ethical review

Group differences in deviance detection

First, we examined group differences in average discrimination between frequent and deviant sounds. MMN and P3a amplitude and latency difference components were computed for each individual within condition. The frequency deviance condition was defined as the response to infrequent tones minus the response to frequent tones and novelty deviance as the response to novel sounds minus the response to frequent tones. As shown in Fig. 1, both groups exhibited classic MMN and P3a deviance responses

Discussion

We used electrophysiology to capture the temporal dynamics of electrophysiological response to auditory deviance in a large, well-characterized sample of youth with ASD. Our results showed that individuals with ASD exhibited enhanced neural responses to auditory deviance detection overall, suggesting a heightened sensitivity to change relative to controls. However, closer examination of dynamic change in this ERP component revealed the overall group difference was actually driven by early

Conclusions

In sum, this is the first large-scale study to characterize the temporal dynamics associated with deviance detection in individuals with ASD. We find that individuals with ASD exhibit a heightened response driven by early detection of frequency deviance (i.e., tone discrimination) and reduced habituation (i.e., prolonged processing of deviance) in response to novel sounds. Our results contribute an important link between the dynamic nature of neural responses and sensory abnormalities,

Conflicts of interest

None.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the children and families for their participation in this study. We would also like to thank the clinicians and coordinators of the Bernier Lab who helped collect clinical and behavioral data, including Ari Wallace, Jennifer Gerdts, Rachel Earl, Jessica Peterson, and Sandy Trinh. This work was supported by the National Institutes of Mental Health [MH100047 Bernier; MH10028 Webb/Bernier], by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development to the University of

References (88)

  • C. Kemner et al.

    Auditory event-related brain potentials in autistic children and three different control groups

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1995)
  • T. Kujala et al.

    Atypical pattern of discriminating sound features in adults with Asperger syndrome as reflected by the mismatch negativity

    Biological Psychology

    (2007)
  • T. Kujala et al.

    Neurophysiological evidence for cortical discrimination impairment of prosody in Asperger syndrome

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2005)
  • T. Lepistö et al.

    The discrimination of and orienting to speech and non-speech sounds in children with autism

    Brain Research

    (2005)
  • T. Lepistö et al.

    Auditory cortical change detection in adults with Asperger syndrome

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2007)
  • T. Lepistö et al.

    Auditory perception and attention as reflected by the brain event-related potentials in children with Asperger syndrome

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2006)
  • R. Näätänen et al.

    The mismatch negativity and its magnetic equivalent: An index of language impairment or more general cognitive decline in autism?

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • R. Näätänen et al.

    The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review

    Clinical Neurophysiology: Official Journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2007)
  • R. Näätänen et al.

    “Primitive intelligence” in the auditory cortex

    Trends in Neurosciences

    (2001)
  • R.A. Poldrack

    Is “efficiency” a useful concept in cognitive neuroscience?

    Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2015)
  • J. Polich

    Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2007)
  • T.P.L. Roberts et al.

    Auditory magnetic mismatch field latency: A biomarker for language impairment in autism

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • C.H. Salmond et al.

    Heterogeneity in the patterns of neural abnormality in autistic spectrum disorders: Evidence from ERP and MRI

    Cortex

    (2007)
  • F. Samson et al.

    Atypical processing of auditory temporal complexity in autistics

    Neuropsychologia

    (2011)
  • L.D. Sculthorpe et al.

    MMN elicitation during natural sleep to violations of an auditory pattern

    Brain Research

    (2009)
  • S. Seri et al.

    Autism in tuberous sclerosis: Evoked potential evidence for a deficit in auditory sensory processing

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1999)
  • A.C. Tsolaki et al.

    Brain source localization of MMN and P300 ERPs in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: A high-density EEG approach

    Neurobiology of Aging

    (2017)
  • U. Volpe et al.

    The cortical generators of P3a and P3b: A LORETA study

    Brain Research Bulletin

    (2007)
  • T.M. Achenbach et al.

    Manual for the child behavior checklist and revised child behavior profile

    (1983)
  • K. Alho

    Cerebral generators of mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes

    Ear and Hearing

    (1995)
  • A.G. Barnett et al.

    Regression to the mean: What it is and how to deal with it

    International Journal of Epidemiology

    (2005)
  • B.A. Boyd et al.

    Sensory features and repetitive behaviors in children with autism and developmental delays

    Autism Research

    (2010)
  • C. Brown et al.

    Adolescent/adult sensory profile manual

    (2002)
  • C. Cantiani et al.

    From sensory perception to lexical-semantic processing: an ERP study in non-verbal children with autism

    PLoS ONE

    (2016)
  • Y.-S. Chang et al.

    Autism and sensory processing disorders: Shared white matter disruption in sensory pathways but divergent connectivity in social-emotional pathways

    PLoS ONE

    (2014)
  • H. Cléry et al.

    Atypical visual change processing in children with autism: An electrophysiological study

    Psychophysiology

    (2013)
  • T. Cui et al.

    P300 amplitude and latency in autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis

    European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

    (2016)
  • R. Čeponienė et al.

    Speech-sound-selective auditory impairment in children with autism: They can perceive but do not attend

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

    (2003)
  • L.Y. Deouell

    The frontal generator of the mismatch negativity revisited

    Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2007)
  • M.A. Dunn et al.

    Mismatch negativity in children with autism and typical development

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2008)
  • Dunn, W. (1999). Sensory profile: Users manual. San...
  • C.D. Elliott

    Differential ability scales-ll

    (2007)
  • C. Escera et al.

    Role of mismatch negativity and novelty-P3 in involuntary auditory attention

    Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2007)
  • M.H. Giard et al.

    Brain generators implicated in the processing of auditory stimulus deviance: A topographic event-related potential study

    Psychophysiology

    (1990)
  • Cited by (53)

    • Understanding sensory regulation in typical and atypical development: The case of sensory seeking

      2022, Developmental Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      By limiting stimulus input, either through reduced seeking of novel stimulation, or by engaging in restricted, repetitive and often self-produced activities, children with ASD would compensate for their sensory hypersensitivity and preserve an optimal level of stimulation. In line with this explanation, increased behavioural (e.g., Baranek et al., 2007) and neural response to sensory stimulation (e.g., Cascio et al., 2012, 2015; Ferri et al., 2003; Hudac et al., 2018; Lepistö et al., 2007, 2008; Miyazaki et al., 2007; Samson et al., 2012; Takarae et al., 2016) were reported in children with ASD. Further, Donkers et al., (2015) documented a relationship between neural hypersensitivity to auditory stimulation (indexed by enhanced P3a responses to novel tones) and lower sensory seeking behaviours (indexed by combined observational and parent-reported indicators) in 4–12 years old children with ASD.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Authors equally contributed as senior author.

    View full text