Elsevier

Brain and Cognition

Volume 83, Issue 3, December 2013, Pages 342-350
Brain and Cognition

Predicting the biological variability of environmental rhythms: Weak or strong anticipation for sensorimotor synchronization?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2013.10.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Sensorimotor synchronization has generally been studied using regular metronomes.

  • Yet natural environmental rhythms exhibit typical fractal variability.

  • We model synchronization processes with various types of (fractal) variable rhythms.

  • Processes are same for all variable rhythms but different than for regular rhythms.

  • The generalizability of results from regular-metronome paradigms is questioned.

Abstract

The internal processes involved in synchronizing our movements with environmental stimuli have traditionally been addressed using regular metronomic sequences. Regarding real-life environments, however, biological rhythms are known to have intrinsic variability, ubiquitously characterized as fractal long-range correlations. In our research we thus investigate to what extent the synchronization processes drawn from regular metronome paradigms can be generalized to other (biologically) variable rhythms. Participants performed synchronized finger tapping under five conditions of long-range and/or short-range correlated, randomly variable, and regular auditory sequences. Combining experimental data analysis and numerical simulation, we found that synchronizing with biologically variable rhythms involves the same internal processes as with other variable rhythms (whether totally random or comprising lawful regularities), but different from those involved with a regular metronome. This challenges both the generalizability of conclusions drawn from regular-metronome paradigms, and recent research assuming that biologically variable rhythms may trigger specific strong anticipatory processes to achieve synchronization.

Introduction

Synchronizing our movements with environmental rhythms such as a partner’s walking cadence is part of our everyday sensorimotor behavior. Among the most striking examples may be our spontaneous tendency to move on the beat of music, observed since the earliest months of life (Zentner & Eerola, 2010). The sensorimotor synchronization paradigm has traditionally investigated the neuropsychological processes involved in synchronizing simple rhythmic movements, such as finger tapping, with basically regular (isochronous) auditory metronomes (Repp, 2005). Despite notable pushes towards studying synchronization with non-isochronous sequences including musical pieces (e.g., Rankin et al., 2009, Repp, 2002), local tempo changes (e.g., Schulze, Cordes, & Vorberg, 2005), and virtual adaptive partner’s movements (e.g., Repp & Keller, 2008), the main focus on regular metronomic sequences however contrasts with the typically variable rhythms contained in environmental stimuli. Especially, it is widely acknowledged that rhythms generated by natural bio-behavioral systems exhibit a characteristic structure of fluctuations over time, namely long-range correlation, or fractal fluctuations (e.g., Bullmore et al., 2009, Gilden, 2009, Kello et al., 2010, Markowitz et al., 2013, West and Schlesinger, 1989). Research has shown that the human perceptual-motor system is sensitive to the temporal structure of biologically variable stimuli (Wark, Lundstrom, & Fairhall, 2007), and synchronizing with fractal signals has recently been assumed to involve a very specific synchronization process, namely Strong Anticipation (Marmelat and Delignières, 2012, Stephen et al., 2008, Stepp and Turvey, 2010).

The strong anticipation hypothesis has been formulated in contrast to more classical conceptions of weak anticipatory processes (Dubois, 2003), involving internal models for short-term prediction and correction of the periods and/or asynchronies produced to achieve synchronization (e.g., Madison and Delignières, 2009, Mates, 1994a, Mates, 1994b, Semjen et al., 2000, Thaut et al., 1998, Vorberg and Schulze, 2002, Vorberg and Wing, 1996; for a review see Repp, 2005, Repp and Su, 2013). In contrast to such local adaptations, strong anticipation is assumed to involve a global tuning of behavior to the statistical properties of environmental fluctuations. This attunement relies on the existence of lawful regularities in environmental fluctuations, hence the role of long-range correlations in the stimuli presented. In this way, the temporal structure of environmental stimuli determines the structure of behavior, leading to the matching of environmental and behavioral fluctuation structures (Stephen et al., 2008).

The Strong Anticipation framework thus questions the generalizability of results from classical synchronization paradigms to ecological, i.e. fractal – or long-range correlated – conditions. In particular it raises the following issues: First, are synchronization processes to be differentiated between long-range correlated stimuli and other variable/regular stimuli, or between any kind of variable (including long-range correlated) stimuli and regular stimuli? Second, to what extent can alternative models, based on local short-term adaptations, account for the matching of fluctuation structures attributed to strong anticipation? These issues have potentially important implications for optimizing human/artificial-environment interaction, like the use of rhythmic auditory stimulation for gait rehabilitation in Parkinson’s patients (e.g., Hove, Suzuki, Uchitomi, Orimo, & Miyake, 2012).

To answer these issues we used a synchronized finger tapping paradigm under five different conditions of variable (long-range and/or short-range correlated, uncorrelated) and regular metronomes. In view of the above mentioned elements we formulated the following working hypotheses:

  • 1.

    Strong anticipation should only be involved in conditions where inter-stimuli intervals comprise long-range correlations, and be unaffected by the presence of other forms of lawful regularities.

  • 2.

    The matching of behavioral and environmental structures of fluctuations should be observed only in conditions where inter-stimuli intervals comprise long-range correlations.

  • 3.

    The structure matching should not be accounted for by simple models implementing local short-term synchronization processes.

Section snippets

Participants and device

Eleven young adults (9 males and 2 females; mean age 30.2 years, ±8) volunteered to participate. Nine declared themselves right-handed, and two left-handed. They had no extensive practice in music, and none of them declared neurological or recent upper-limb injury which might affect finger tapping performance. Informed consent was obtained from participants, and the study protocol was approved by the local institutional review board (Montpellier-1 University).

Participants were seated comfortably

Synchronization performance

Fig. 1 displays an example of IOI and the corresponding ITI series produced in a single experimental trial. Participants showed similar synchronization performance in all variable and regular metronome conditions. In particular, results demonstrated the typical negative mean asynchrony (mean ASYN = −53 ms, ±33 ms) commonly reported in literature on sensorimotor synchronization (Repp, 2005), and a one-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed no significant difference in mean asynchrony between the five

Modeling: local synchronization processes as an alternative to strong anticipation

On account of the strong interdependence between inter-onset intervals, asynchronies, and inter-tap intervals within a same synchronization trial, the positive cross-correlation evidenced between ITIn and IOIn−1 (Fig. 4) could be the effect of (i) correction of the just-produced asynchrony by lengthening or shortening current ITI (e.g., Vorberg & Schulze’s (2002) linear phase correction model), or (ii) current ITI reproducing the length of preceding IOI, or (iii) a combination of both

Discussion

The aim of this work was to examine whether synchronizing with a biologically variable (long-range correlated) environmental rhythm involves a specific internal process, notably strong anticipation, differing from those involved with other forms of variable or regular rhythms. In this view we asked in particular whether models of local short-term adaptations allow to provide an alternative account for the matching of the serial correlation structures in inter-tap and inter-onset intervals.

Conclusion and perspectives

Taken together, experimental and modeling results of the present study lead to conclude that synchronizing with a biologically variable, i.e. long-range correlated rhythm involves the same internal processes as with other variable rhythms, whether presenting lawful regularities or not. These processes are however different from those involved with a regular rhythm. On the one hand, our results thus challenge to some extent the generalizability of conclusions drawn from regular-metronome

References (38)

  • B.J. West et al.

    The living matter way to exchange information

    Medical Hypotheses

    (2010)
  • J. Beran

    Statistics for long-memory processes

    (1994)
  • J.E. Cutting et al.

    Attention and the evolution of hollywood film

    Psychological Science

    (2010)
  • D. Delignières et al.

    Long-range correlation in synchronization and syncopation tapping: A linear phase correction model

    PLoS One

    (2009)
  • D.M. Dubois

    Mathematical foundations of discrete and functional systems with strong and weak anticipations

    Lecture Notes in Computer Science

    (2003)
  • D.L. Gilden

    Global model analysis of cognitive variability

    Cognitive Science

    (2009)
  • D.L. Gilden et al.

    1/f noise in human cognition

    Science

    (1995)
  • H. Hennig et al.

    The nature and perception of fluctuations in human musical rhythm

    PLoS One

    (2010)
  • M.J. Hove et al.

    Interactive rhythmic auditory stimulation reinstates natural 1/f timing in gait of Parkinson’s patients

    PLoS One

    (2012)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text