Elsevier

Brain and Cognition

Volume 71, Issue 3, December 2009, Pages 246-258
Brain and Cognition

Mechanisms of deficit of visuospatial attention shift in children with developmental coordination disorder: A neurophysiological measure of the endogenous Posner paradigm

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.006Get rights and content

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to investigate and compare the mechanisms of brain activity, as revealed by a combination of the visuospatial attention shifting paradigm and event-related potentials (ERP) in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) and typically developing children. Twenty-eight DCD children and 26 typically developing children were recorded with regard to their behavioral performance and ERP measures during a variant of the endogenous Posner paradigm, in which they should react to visual targets preceded by spatial cues or presented uncued. Children with DCD showed longer reaction time and a deficit in inhibitory response capacity when compared to typically developing children. The electrophysiological characteristics also showed distinct modulatory effects upon attentional orienting, anticipatory mechanisms, and cognitive-to-motor transfer in children with DCD: longer cue-P3 and target-N1 latency, smaller target-P3 amplitude, an elongated interval between N2 and the motor response (N2-RT), and small areas on contingent negative variation (CNV). The combined analysis of behavioral performance and ERP data suggested that children with DCD had deficits of slower target identification (N1), less ability in interhemispheric (P3) and cognitive-to-motor transfer speed (N2-RT), as well as a less mature anticipatory and executive process (CNV).

Introduction

Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD), in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorder (DSM-IV) of the American Psychiatric Association and the ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioral Disorder in Children and Adolescents from the World Health Organization, is a term related to poor motor proficiency that is not the result of specific neurological diseases or other psychiatric, physical, or medical problems (American Psychiatric Association, 1994, WHO, 1992, WHO, 1996). In spite of the absence of any other disorder, DCD interferes significantly with the child’s academic achievement and normal daily activities (Henderson & Hall, 1982). More importantly, DCD has been demonstrated to often persist into adolescence and early adulthood, along with associated psycho-social problems (Cantell et al., 1994, Visser et al., 1998).

Although the explicitly etiological significance of DCD is still unclear, the definite deficits demonstrated are motor control impairments (Cherng et al., 2007, Tsai et al., 2008), neural constraints (Sigmundsson et al., 1997a, Sigmundsson et al., 1997b, Sigmundsson et al., 1999), and more neurological soft signs (Lundy-Ekman, Ivry, Keele, & Woollacott, 1991) in children with DCD as compared to typically developing children. Some researchers from the neurobehavioral models of inter- and intra-sensory functioning have formulated hypotheses that the mechanism of DCD seems to be related to right hemisphere insufficiency or dysfunctional corpus callosum (Sigmundsson et al., 1997a, Sigmundsson et al., 1997b, Sigmundsson et al., 1999). Neuropsychological and psychological studies have also made an effort to find out whether DCD is specifically impaired in motor function or in isolated cognitive processing, or is involved in both cognitive and motor disorders. Although the results are still inconclusive, numerous studies have suggested that children with DCD display deficits in perceptual or perceptual-motor functioning (Bonifacci, 2004, Lord and Hulme, 1987, Lord and Hulme, 1988, Sigmundsson et al., 2003, Tsai et al., 2008, Tsai and Wu, 2008, Van Waelvelde et al., 2004, Wilson and McKenzie, 1998). Furthermore, it has been suggested that the mechanism of impairments of motor coordination is most pronounced on and strongly associated with low-level perceptual function, especially with a deficiency related to visuospatial information processing (Tsai and Wu, 2008, Tsai et al., 2008, Wilson and McKenzie, 1998).

Since the neuroanatomical bases of dysfunction in children with DCD are indistinct, speculations on the potential neuropathology can utilize theories of the cognitive anatomy of attention (Swanson et al., 1991). As a result, different versions of covert orienting of endogenous and exogenous visuospatial attention tasks (e.g., the Posner paradigm) with different stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) (Tsai et al., 2009, Wilson and Maruff, 1999, Wilson et al., 1997), the classic Simon task (Mandich et al., 2002, Tsai et al., 2009), and some visuospatial cue tasks (Mandich et al., 2003, Wilmut et al., 2007) have been employed with the base on the neuropsychological domain in order to explore the potential mechanism of motor deficits. Although the general slowing of responses best reflects the motor impairments that occur as part of DCD, children with DCD display a selective visuospatial deficit only in shifts of volitional (endogenous) but not dislocation of automatic (exogenous) attention (Mandich et al., 2002, Mandich et al., 2003, Tsai et al., 2009, Wilson and Maruff, 1999, Wilson et al., 1997).

The Posner paradigm (Posner, 1980), owing to an important characteristic of attentional control within the region of visual space in the absence of eye movements and with minimal motor element involvement, has been considered to appropriately assess the deficit of visuospatial information processing in children with DCD (Tsai, 2009, Tsai et al., 2009, Wilson and Maruff, 1999, Wilson et al., 1997). In order to produce efficient behavior during performance of the Posner paradigm task, subjects have to apply selective attention regarding the plastic ability to prioritize processing of certain stimulus features (Correa, Lupianez, Madrid, & Tudela, 2006). In covert orienting of a visuospatial attention task in the endogenous Posner paradigm, children are asked to react to visual target stimulus after centrally delivered cues pointing to the correct target side (i.e., valid condition) or the opposite side (i.e., invalid condition) (Posner and Cohen, 1984, Wilson and Maruff, 1999, Wilson et al., 1997). In some of the trials, no directional information related to the two potential locations for the target is given, to keep attention diffused and alert in the neutral (non-cued) condition. The endogenous Posner paradigm can be interpreted at a cognitive level and procedurally motivate volitional/intentional movement, and is also more dependent on the activation of cortical attention areas, such as posterior parietal cortex and frontal lobes (Posner and Cohen, 1984, Wilson et al., 1997). This endogenous facilitation seems to result in widespread cortical activation to attain volitionally generated shifts of attentional resources (Mayer, Dorflinger, Rao, & Seidenberg, 2004). Since the attention network test involves alerting, orienting, and executive attention, which are associated with different specific brain areas, the endogenous Posner paradigm can efficiently assess individual difference in cerebral responses (Fan et al., 2002, Posner et al., 2007).

ERP recordings made during the Posner paradigm task performance permitted on-line measures of shifts of attention in the order of milliseconds, as well as the assessment of the consequences of visual and motor processing of the stimuli, which cannot be obtained by behavioral performance alone (Purves et al., 2008). A network of brain areas used in focusing visual attention to spatial locations has been discovered (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002, Nobre, 2001, Perchet and Garcia-Larrea, 2000, Perchet and Garcia-Larrea, 2005), demonstrating early electrophysiological activity over the occipital region for perceptual processing and late electrophysiological activity related to decision-making and motor processes (Mangun, 1995). Furthermore, the posterior parietal region is concerned with covert orienting and the redirecting of it (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002, Rushworth et al., 2003). Some researchers have found that children aged 6–9, when performing the exogenous Posner task, already have well developed simpler cognitive processes with the ERP reflections (Perchet and Garcia-Larrea, 2000, Perchet and Garcia-Larrea, 2005). Therefore, within a single recording session, the possibility of examining the ERP concomitants of different cognitive axes – anticipatory processing prior to actual target stimulus, attentional priming of different cued stimuli, target identification, and response selection – seems to be warranted in children.

The endogenous visuospatial attention task coupled with ERP recording have been demonstrated to evoke the different visual response components on ERP: early visual responses N1 modulation reflect stimulus triggered orienting and engagement of voluntary visuospatial attention to relevant stimulus locations (Fu et al., 2008, Griffin et al., 2002, Luck et al., 1990), and late potential activities, such as N2 for response inhibition processes (Kok, 1986) and P300 (P3) for cognitive responses (i.e., motor processes) (Griffin et al., 2002, Linden, 2005).

In sum, notwithstanding the behavior deficit of inhibitory response capacity during completion of endogenous visuospatial attention orienting tasks which has been demonstrated in children with DCD (Mandich et al., 2002, Tsai, 2009, Tsai et al., 2009, Wilson and Maruff, 1999, Wilson et al., 1997), to the best of our knowledge, the mechanisms of both early perceptual and late motor processing in brain activity underlying such behavior anomalies have not yet been explored. Meanwhile, as described above, analysis of ERP elicited by the stimuli of cue and target should reflect the dynamics of brain activity related to the orienting of attention in time. Furthermore, it is worth mentioning that the effect sizes of correlations of motor and cognitive deficits were significant only for demanding timed motor-response items, the magnitude of which was markedly higher than that for untimed items in children with DCD (Van Waelvelde et al., 2004, Wilson and McKenzie, 1998). Taken together, the purpose of this study was to simultaneously assess the ERP and behavioral performances in children with DCD when performing the endogenous Posner paradigm task with SOA 350 ms, as compared to those of age-matched control subjects. This was not only because this topic has not previously been addressed, but also because the results from synchronous assessment of the neuropsychological and cognitive electrophysiological data can provide more compelling evidence and gain deeper insight into abnormal behavior performances. In addition, the present study can offer information on how the attentional-orienting mechanism modulates stimulus processing in the endogenous Posner paradigm task in typically developing children.

Section snippets

Subjects

Two hundred and seventy-two school children aged 9–10 years were recruited from mainstream classrooms in various regions of Taiwan. None of the children selected in the current study had any definite signs of neurological disorders or behavioral problems, or special needs in education that would qualify as exclusionary criteria for this study. The DCD selection procedure was carried out using a previously validated method (Tsai et al., 2009, Tsai, 2009, Tsai et al., 2009). Each child was

Results

The two groups, 28 children with DCD (12 boys and 16 girls) and 26 controls (12 boys and 14 girls), were matched at the group level on age [t(52) = −0.40, p = .693], and IQ values [t(52) = −1.23, p = .208] (see Table 1).

Discussion

The reliable group main effect of the endogenous visuospatial attention emerged in the current study, with children with DCD showing longer RT than control children. This suggests that children with DCD displayed a generalized reduction in the time efficiency of the central processing of cognitive functions. More importantly, children with DCD did indeed display a deficit with regard to the volitional movement of attention, which corroborates the results obtained in previous studies (Mandich et

Conclusions

Children with DCD responded significantly more slowly than typically developing children and exhibited a deficit of inhibitory response capacity in the endogenous mode of orienting attention. Analysis of ERP elicited by the cue and target stimuli did provide more direct measures of processing on the timing and the pattern of brain activity with regard to attention-focus, response and inhibition processes over time. Indeed, the current findings give some indication of the core deficits

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for the participation of students and staff in this study, which also supported by Grants from the National Science Council (NSC 96-2413-H-006-011) and National Cheng Kung University (D97-3400) in Taiwan.

References (97)

  • S.J. Luck et al.

    Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. II. Functional dissociation of P1 and N1 components

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1990)
  • S.J. Luck et al.

    Event-related potential studies of attention

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2000)
  • A. Mandich et al.

    On the ability of children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) to inhibit response initiation: The Simon effect

    Brain and Cognition

    (2002)
  • A. Mandich et al.

    Children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) and their ability to disengage ongoing attentional focus: More on inhibitory function

    Brain and Cognition

    (2003)
  • N.C. Martin et al.

    DCD and ADHD: A genetic study of their shared aetiology

    Human Movement Science

    (2006)
  • A.R. Mayer et al.

    Neural networks underlying endogenous and exogenous visual-spatial orienting

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • M.A. Mon-Williams et al.

    Visual evoked potentials in children with developmental coordination disorder

    Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics

    (1996)
  • A.C. Nobre

    The attentive homunculus: now you see it, now you don’t

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2001)
  • C. Perchet et al.

    Learning to react: Anticipatory mechanisms in children and adults during a visuospatial attention task

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2005)
  • C. Perchet et al.

    Attention shifts and anticipatory mechanisms in hyperactive children: An ERP study using the Posner paradigm

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • J. Polich et al.

    P300 as a clinical assay: Rationale, evaluation, and findings

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2000)
  • J.R. Ramautar et al.

    Effects of stop-signal modality on the N2/P3 complex elicited in the stop-signal paradigm

    Biological Psychology

    (2006)
  • J.W. Rohrbaugh et al.

    Sensory and motor aspects of the contingent negative variation

  • H. Sigmundsson et al.

    Do ‘clumsy’ children have visual deficits?

    Behavioural Brain Research

    (2003)
  • A. Starr et al.

    Readiness to response in a target detection task: Pre- and post-stimulus event-related potentials in normal subjects

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1995)
  • C.L. Tsai

    The effectiveness of exercise intervention on inhibitory control in children with developmental coordination disorder: Using a visuospatial attention paradigm as a model

    Research in Developmental Disabilities

    (2009)
  • C.L. Tsai et al.

    Role of visual perceptual skills (non-motor) in children with developmental coordination disorder

    Human Movement Science

    (2008)
  • C.L. Tsai et al.

    Static balance in children with developmental coordination disorder

    Human Movement Science

    (2008)
  • C.L. Tsai et al.

    Inhibitory response capacities of bilateral lower and upper extremities in children with developmental coordination disorder in endogenous and exogenous orienting modes

    Brain and Cognition

    (2009)
  • V. van Veen et al.

    The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies

    Physiology and Behavior

    (2002)
  • J. Visser et al.

    The relationship between physical growth, movement experience and motor skills in adolescence. Differences between children with DCD and controls

    Human Movement Science

    (1998)
  • P.H. Wilson et al.

    Deficits in the endogenous control of covert visuospatial attention in children with developmental coordination disorder

    Human Movement Science

    (1999)
  • American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington,...
  • R.H. Bruininks et al.

    Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency

    (2005)
  • C.H. Brunia

    Waiting in readiness: Gating in attention and motor preparation

    Psychophysiology

    (1993)
  • M.H. Cantell et al.

    Clumsiness in adolescence. Educational, motor, and social outcomes of motor delay detected at five years

    Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly

    (1994)
  • M. Corbetta et al.

    Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain

    Nature Reviews, Neuroscience

    (2002)
  • C.C. Duncan-Johnson et al.

    On quantifying surprise: The variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability

    Psychophysiology

    (1977)
  • G.J. Dupaul et al.

    ADHD rating scale II: checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation

    (1998)
  • M. Eimer

    ERP modulations indicate the selective processing of visual stimuli as a result of transient and sustained spatial attention

    Psychophysiology

    (1996)
  • M. Eimer

    Mechanisms of visuospatial attention: Evidence from event-related potentials

    Visual Cognition

    (1998)
  • A. Facoetti et al.

    Orienting of visual attention in dyslexia: Evidence for asymmetric hemispheric control of attention

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2001)
  • J. Fan et al.

    Testing the efficiency and independence of attention networks

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2002)
  • M.D. Fox et al.

    Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2006)
  • J.M. Fuster

    Executive frontal functions

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2000)
  • L. Garcia-Larrea et al.

    Interference of cellular phone conversations with visuomotor tasks: An ERP study

    Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2001)
  • D. Goddin et al.

    IFCN recommended standards for long-latency auditory event-related potentials. Report of an IFCN committee. International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1994)
  • C.M. Gornez et al.

    Fronto-parietal networks activation during the contingent negative variation period

    Brain Research Bulletin

    (2007)
  • Cited by (54)

    • Reduced relative volume in motor and attention regions in developmental coordination disorder: A voxel-based morphometry study

      2017, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Support for deficits in these executive functioning processes in children with DCD is evident from behavioral research using a broad range of paradigms (e.g., visual and verbal working memory tasks, n-back paradigms, endogenous Posner paradigm, dual-task performance; Wilson et al., 2013). Furthermore, at a neurological level, differences in attentional network connectivity (Querne et al., 2008), functional activation patterns (Zwicker et al., 2010), and electrophysiological event related potential measures during spatial working memory (Tsai et al., 2012) and visuospatial attention (Tsai et al., 2009) EEG tasks have also been identified in children with DCD. The identified reductions in GM volume are consistent with behavioral research demonstrating motor planning and execution, attentional, and executive functioning deficits associated with DCD.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text