Elsevier

Brain and Cognition

Volume 69, Issue 1, February 2009, Pages 56-64
Brain and Cognition

Brain activation and deactivation during location and color working memory tasks in 11–13-year-old children

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2008.05.010Get rights and content

Abstract

Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and n-back tasks we investigated whether, in 11–13-year-old children, spatial (location) and nonspatial (color) information is differentially processed during visual attention (0-back) and working memory (WM) (2-back) tasks and whether such cognitive task performance, compared to a resting state, results in regional deactivation. The location 0-back task, compared to the color 0-back task, activated segregated areas in the frontal, parietal and occipital cortices whereas no differentially activated voxels were obtained when location and color 2-back tasks were directly contrasted. Several midline cortical areas were less active during 0- and 2-back task performance than resting state. The task-induced deactivation increased with task difficulty as demonstrated by larger deactivation during 2-back than 0-back tasks. The results suggest that, in 11–13-year-old children, the visual attentional network is differently recruited by spatial and nonspatial information processing, but the functional organization of cortical activation in WM in this age group is not based on the type of information processed. Furthermore, 11–13-year-old children exhibited a similar pattern of cortical deactivation that has been reported in adults during cognitive task performance compared to a resting state.

Introduction

Working memory (WM) refers to temporary storage and manipulation of information in the service of goal-directed behavior (Baddeley, 1992, Baddeley, 1996). This function is essential, e.g., in learning, reasoning, problem solving and language comprehension. WM performance and the underlying neural processes and brain structures continue to develop during childhood until adolescence and young adulthood (Gogtay et al., 2004, Hale et al., 1997, Luciana and Nelson, 1998, Luna et al., 2004, Luna et al., 2001).

Visual WM has been extensively studied in adults using brain imaging methods whereas only a small number of studies have been conducted in children. Brain imaging studies in adults have shown that regions in the prefrontal (PFC) and posterior parietal (PPC) cortices are central to visual WM (Klingberg, 2006, Owen et al., 2005). Most fMRI studies in children have investigated the development of visuospatial WM (Klingberg et al., 2002, Kwon et al., 2002, Nelson et al., 2000, Olesen et al., 2007, Scherf et al., 2006, Thomas et al., 1999), and in only a few studies the development of nonspatial WM was investigated (Casey et al., 1995, Ciesielski et al., 2006, Crone et al., 2006). These studies suggest that areas of the frontoparietal network that are activated in adults are also recruited in children in the performance of visual WM tasks, although the degree of engagement of different regions may change with maturation (Berl, Vaidya, & Gaillard, 2006). Furthermore, the development of visuospatial (Klingberg et al., 2002, Kwon et al., 2002) and nonspatial (Crone et al., 2006) WM capacity has been shown to be related to increased activity in the frontoparietal cortical areas and to maturation of the frontoparietal white matter (Olesen, Nagy, Westerberg, & Klingberg, 2003). Children’s lower WM capacity has been attributed to the maturational phase of the PFC. According to pediatric neuroimaging (Giedd et al., 1999, Gogtay et al., 2004) and post-mortem studies (Chugani et al., 1987, Huttenlocher, 1979, Huttenlocher, 1990, Huttenlocher, 1997), the PFC is among the brain regions that continue to mature until early adulthood.

The PFC has a central role in WM (Fuster, 1989, Goldman-Rakic, 1987), and several models have been proposed to explain the functional organization of this cortical area in WM (reviewed e.g. by Fletcher & Henson, 2001). In adults, functional imaging studies of the “process-specific” model provide evidence that the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) is responsible for monitoring and manipulating and the ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC) for maintaining information in WM (D’Esposito et al., 1999, D’Esposito et al., 2000, Owen, 2000, Owen et al., 1998, Petrides, 2005, Petrides et al., 2002, Rypma, 2006). Another model, called the domain-specific model (Goldman-Rakic, 1995, Wilson et al., 1993) has also gained support from brain imaging studies demonstrating that WM processing of spatial information is handled in the DLPFC and nonspatial information in the VLPFC (Belger et al., 1998, Courtney et al., 1996, Mottaghy et al., 2002, Sala et al., 2003, Ventre-Dominey et al., 2005). The two models may not be mutually exclusive, however, as there is evidence that the organization of the PFC is based both on the type of information being processed and the cognitive process employed during the performance of WM tasks (Johnson et al., 2003, Mohr et al., 2006).

WM and attention are closely intertwined; both processes promote goal-directed behavior and show a considerable overlap of the underlying neural circuitry (Awh et al., 2006, Chun and Turk-Browne, 2007, LaBar et al., 1999, Wager et al., 2004). Attentional systems have also been divided into spatial and nonspatial domains. The results of several neuroimaging studies suggest that top-down control of spatial and nonspatial attention activates largely similar areas of the frontoparietal network but subregions of this network are specific for controlling spatial selective attention (Coull and Frith, 1998, Giesbrecht et al., 2003, Vandenberghe et al., 2001).

In the adult brain, in the absence of cognitive task performance, i.e., during a resting state, several areas of the brain are active (Gusnard and Raichle, 2001, McKiernan et al., 2003, Raichle et al., 2001). Evidence is accumulating that the spontaneous, resting-state activity that can be measured with functional brain imaging reflects fluctuations of specifically organized activity between regions that have similar functionality (for a review see Fox & Raichle 2007). Resting-state activity has also been reported in children (Kiviniemi et al., 2000), infants (Fransson et al., 2007) and monkeys (Vincent et al., 2007). The “default mode network” refers to areas in the medial prefrontal and parietal cortices, cingulate cortex and inferior parietal lobule that are active during the resting-state and are attenuated (deactivated) during attention demanding goal-directed task performance (Raichle et al., 2001). Activity observed in the default mode network has been suggested to reflect internally directed mental processes (Gusnard & Raichle, 2001) and mind-wandering (Mason et al., 2007).

The physiological mechanisms underlying deactivation are incompletely understood at present. Deactivation seen by fMRI is suggested to result from disproportionate decrease in blood flow and glucose utilization (Raichle & Mintun, 2006) and may reflect a decrease in the activity of the cells that project to the deactivated area rather than increased activity of local inhibitory interneurons in that area (Gusnard & Raichle, 2001). Deactivation has been reported almost exclusively in adults with one exception reported in infants (Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2006) and another in adolescents (Schweinsburg, Nagel, & Tapert, 2005).

In the present study, we investigate whether attention and WM processing of spatial and nonspatial information in children aged 11–13 is segregated as suggested by domain-specific model. To our knowledge, there are no earlier brain imaging studies that have investigated the organization of spatial and nonspatial WM processing in the child brain. We used location and color 0- and 2-back tasks and a resting state and fMRI to locate the brain areas displaying visuospatial and nonspatial attention and WM related activation and deactivation.

The 2-back task requires on-line monitoring, updating, and manipulation of retained information placing great demands on the key processes within WM whereas the 0-back is an attentional task that requires detection of a predetermined stimulus but, contrary to the 2-back task, does not demand manipulation or memorizing of the stimuli presented earlier (Owen et al., 2005). In designing the experiment, we specifically attempted to get similar difficulty levels for the location and color attention and WM tasks. To minimize the effect of age and gender on the WM performance (Vuontela et al., 2003), and to keep age- (Berl et al., 2006) and gender-related (Schweinsburg et al., 2005) differences in brain activation patterns as small as possible the subject group consisted of girls within a narrow age range of 11–13 years. Based on our earlier studies (Vuontela et al., 2003) we chose to investigate this age group that mastered the tasks relatively well compared to younger children. Furthermore, the results of event-related fMRI studies of spatial WM (Klingberg et al., 2002, Kwon et al., 2002) have suggested that WM networks in children within this age group are still in the process of maturation.

In the present experiment, we employed the 0-back that is an attentional task to enable the detection of possible difference between the neural networks involved in spatial and nonspatial attention. On the basis of the adult neuroimaging data (Giesbrecht et al., 2003, Vandenberghe et al., 2001), we predicted that spatial and nonspatial attention would recruit partially segregated regions of the frontoparietal attentional network in children. In any case, we expected to see this difference in the neural network of the PPC that matures earlier than the PFC network (Gogtay et al., 2004). The 2-back task was employed to test the domain-specific model of mnemonic processing. Considering the adult neuroimaging data, visual WM processing in children is as likely to be organized according to either of the two alternative models of WM. The resting state was included in the experiment to give children a break in between the task performance and to test whether deactivation mechanisms found in adult subjects induced by cognitive task performance are functional in 11–13-year-old children.

Section snippets

Subjects

Nine girls aged 11–13 years (mean = 12.2), all healthy and right-handed, participated in the experiment. All subjects were Caucasian, of Finnish nationality and attended normal schools in the Helsinki area. The youngest subject was excluded because of extensive head movement during imaging. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant and the parents of the children. The parents of seven children were classified as belonging to socioeconomic class I and of two children to class II

Behavioral performance

The mean RTs and accuracy levels are shown in Table 1. No significant differences were found between the performance levels of the corresponding location and color n-back tasks as measured by the RTs and accuracy level. The RTs showed a significant main effect of Load (F(1, 7) = 11.80, P < .05): they were longer in the 2-back than 0-back tasks both in the location and color condition (P < .05). Also the accuracy level showed a main effect of Load (F(1, 7) = 7.72, P < .05): the accuracy was lower in the

Discussion

The location and color tasks were performed equally well as indicated by the RTs, incorrect responses and subjective evaluation of their difficulty level. It is not easy to adjust the task difficulty levels because the scanner noise may affect the performance during the imaging. Probably for this reason the performance of children in several earlier studies has been variable or low (Casey et al., 1995, Kwon et al., 2002, Nelson et al., 2000, Thomas et al., 1999). As in our earlier studies using

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Sami Martinkauppi and Juha Koivisto for their assistance in the practical aspects of this work and Ilkka Linnankoski for reviewing the language. The study was supported by grants from the Academy of Finland (National Center of Excellence Program, Chinese-Finnish Neuro Program), the Radiological Society of Finland, the Helsinki University Central Hospital Research Grant (EVO-TYH 4217), Helsinki University Research Funds, Clinical Research Institute Helsinki University

References (93)

  • P.S. Goldman-Rakic

    Cellular basis of working memory

    Neuron

    (1995)
  • M.A. Goodale et al.

    Separate visual pathways for perception and action

    Trends in Neuroscience

    (1992)
  • H. Hautzel et al.

    Topographic segregation and convergence of verbal, object, shape and spatial working memory in humans

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2002)
  • R.N. Henson et al.

    Recoding, storage, rehearsal and grouping in verbal short-term memory: An fMRI study

    Neuropsychologia

    (2000)
  • P.R. Huttenlocher

    Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development

    Neuropsychologia

    (1990)
  • M. Jenkinson et al.

    A Global optimization method for robust affine registration of brain images

    Medical Image Analysis

    (2001)
  • M. Jenkinson et al.

    Improved optimisation for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • B.A. Kirchhoff et al.

    Functional-anatomic correlates of individual differences in memory

    Neuron

    (2006)
  • T. Klingberg

    Development of a superior frontal-intraparietal network for visuo-spatial working memory

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • K.S. LaBar et al.

    Neuroanatomic overlap of working memory and spatial attention networks: A functional MRI comparison within subjects

    NeuroImage

    (1999)
  • M. Luciana et al.

    The functional emergence of prefrontally-guided working memory systems in four- to eight-year old children

    Neuropsychologia

    (1998)
  • B. Luna et al.

    Maturation of widely distributed brain function subserves cognitive development

    NeuroImage

    (2001)
  • M. Mishkin et al.

    Object vision and spatial vision: Two cortical pathways

    Trends in Neuroscience

    (1983)
  • F.M. Mottaghy

    Interfering with working memory in humans

    Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • P.J. Olesen et al.

    Combined analysis of DTI and fMRI data reveals a joint maturation of white and grey matter in a fronto-parietal network

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (2003)
  • B.R. Postle et al.

    An fMRI investigation of cortical contributions to spatial and nonspatial visual working memory

    NeuroImage

    (2000)
  • B. Rypma

    Factors controlling neural activity during delayed-response task performance: Testing a memory organization hypothesis of prefrontal function

    Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • P. Rämä et al.

    Working memory of identification of emotional vocal expressions: An fMRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2001)
  • J.B. Sala et al.

    Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • K.M. Thomas et al.

    A developmental functional MRI study of spatial working memory

    NeuroImage

    (1999)
  • N. Tzourio-Mazoyer et al.

    Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • R. Vandenberghe et al.

    Location- or feature-based targeting of peripheral attention

    NeuroImage

    (2001)
  • D.C. Van Essen et al.

    Hierarchial organization and functional streams in the visual cortex

    Trends in Neuroscience

    (1983)
  • J. Ventre-Dominey et al.

    Double dissociation in neural correlates of visual working memory: A PET study

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (2005)
  • T.D. Wager et al.

    Neuroimaging studies of shifting attention: A meta-analysis

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • M.W. Woolrich et al.

    Multi-level linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • M.W. Woolrich et al.

    Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modelling of fMRI data

    NeuroImage

    (2001)
  • T.M. Achenbach

    Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR and TRF profiles

    (1991)
  • A.D. Baddeley

    Working memory

    Science

    (1992)
  • A.D. Baddeley

    The fractionation of working memory

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (1996)
  • A. Belger et al.

    Dissociation of mnemonic and perceptual processes during spatial and nonspatial working memory using fMRI

    Human Brain Mapping

    (1998)
  • S. Carlson et al.

    Distribution of cortical activation during visuospatial n-back tasks as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging

    Cerebral Cortex

    (1998)
  • H.T. Chugani et al.

    Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development

    Annals of Neurology

    (1987)
  • S.M. Courtney et al.

    Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex

    Cerebral Cortex

    (1996)
  • E.A. Crone et al.

    Neurocognitive development of the ability to manipulate information in working memory

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2006)
  • G. Dehaene-Lambertz et al.

    Functional organization of perisylvian activation during presentation of sentences in preverbal infants

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2006)
  • Cited by (23)

    • Implicit memory of locations and identities: A developmental study

      2018, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Therefore, contextual cueing can be used to evaluate the research question in these age groups. Second, explicit memory research suggests that the age range of 6–10 years reflects an important transition period for the successful differentiation of identity and location information processing (e.g., Booth et al., 2000; Lange-Küttner & Küttner, 2015; Nelson et al., 2000; Passarotti et al., 2003; Thomas et al., 1999; Vuontela et al., 2009). Hence, it is reasonable to expect that if age-related differences in the processing of identity and location information are to be found in implicit learning, we would observe them during this developmental period.

    • Responsiveness and functional connectivity of the scene-sensitive retrosplenial complex in 7-11-year-old children

      2014, Brain and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      The whole-brain analysis showed a group × task interaction in regions of the medial prefrontal, anterior cingular and subcallosal cortices that have earlier been implicated in performance monitoring and optimization, and in response selection processes (Luna, Padmanabhan, & O’Hearn, 2010; Polli et al., 2005; Rushworth, Buckley, Behrens, Walton, & Bannerman, 2007). In line with the findings indicating that these regions show signal decreases during cognitive task performance (Thomason et al., 2008; Vuontela et al., 2009), the mean BOLD signal in the three ROIs corresponding to these areas was negative during the performance of the Sf and Fs tasks in children and adults. However, task-dependent modulation of activity, i.e. the BOLD signal difference between the Sf and Fs tasks in these regions was larger in adults than children.

    • Probing the early development of visual working memory capacity with functional near-infrared spectroscopy

      2014, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      VWM is a core cognitive system with severe capacity limitations that are predictive of individual differences in general cognitive processing (Gold et al., 2010). Recent studies using fMRI have revealed a frontal–parietal network that underlies VWM in both children and adults (e.g., Bunge and Wright, 2007; Edin et al., 2007; Fair et al., 2007; Geier et al., 2009; Klingberg, 2006; Klingberg et al., 2002; Kwon et al., 2002; Mitchell and Cusack, 2007; Nelson et al., 2000; Olesen et al., 2007; Scherf et al., 2006; Thomas et al., 1999; Todd and Marois, 2004, 2005; Todd et al., 2005; Vuontela et al., 2009). The current study is the first to examine whether key signatures of this VWM system are evident in early development using fNIRS.

    • Regulation of brain activity in the fusiform face and parahippocampal place areas in 7-11-year-old children

      2013, Brain and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      The different results may be explained by the age ranges of the children and the experimental designs that differed between the studies. We used relatively simple 1-back tasks in a blocked design that has proved to be a suitable tool for investigating cognitive processes in school-aged children in our prior studies (Vuontela et al., 2003, 2009). There were no correlations between the age, gender, and behavioral performance and the FFA responses in children, but in adults, the RTs correlated positively with the level of signal change in the rFFA in the Sf task.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    These authors contributed equally.

    View full text