Elsevier

Brain and Cognition

Volume 65, Issue 1, October 2007, Pages 36-46
Brain and Cognition

Affective and neuropsychological correlates of children’s rituals and compulsive-like behaviors: Continuities and discontinuities with obsessive–compulsive disorder

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.02.007Get rights and content

Abstract

This study explored the relations among ritualistic and compulsive-like behavior, fears, and neuropsychological performance in typically developing children between the ages of four and eight years. Forty-two children were administered a battery of neuropsychological tasks assessing response inhibition and set-shifting. Two parent-report questionnaires assessed the intensity of children’s fears and compulsive-like behaviors (“just right” perceptions and repetitive behaviors). For younger children (⩽72 months), set-shifting and response inhibition accounted for significant variance in their ritualistic, compulsive-like behaviors. For older children (>72 months), a combination of neuropsychological (response inhibition) and affective (animal fears and social anxiety) factors predicted compulsive-like behaviors. These findings suggest that common neuropsychological mechanisms underlie compulsive, ritualistic behavior exhibited in normal development and in obsessive–compulsive disorder.

Introduction

Routines, rituals and habits represent defining features of human behavior and individual adaptation, and yet, in other contexts, similar behaviors signify severe disturbance. In this paper, we continue our exploration of the normative aspects of rituals and other compulsive-like behaviors in children, drawing parallels between the phenomenology of children’s rituals and those found in obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). We also go beyond phenomenological description and explore the cognitive and neuropsychological aspects of normative compulsive-like behaviors. We conclude by presenting a brain–behavior model that encompasses both adaptive and maladaptive variants of compulsive, ritualistic behavior.

Ritualistic and repetitive behaviors are prevalent throughout the lifespan but are particularly salient in early childhood (Evans et al., 1997, Leonard et al., 1990). Beginning around two or three years of age, children exhibit marked compulsive behavior that includes strong preferences for wholeness and symmetry in the environment, circumscribed and ritualized behaviors, rigidity of likes and dislikes, and heightened sensitivity to minute details (Gesell, 1928, Gesell et al., 1974). Children may focus their attention on minute objects such as flecks of dust or small imperfections in clothes. Likewise, they may order and arrange objects in precise, circumscribed ways or insist that a parent read the same book over and over in a particular manner (Evans et al., 1997, Leonard et al., 1990). Typically waning by age eight, compulsive-like activity reflects a common and normative part of the child’s behavioral repertoire (Evans et al., 1997, Zohar and Felz, 2001).

Although many theorists have recognized that children’s rituals and compulsions are a normal part of development, little is known about the etiology or function of children’s compulsive-like behaviors. Like clinically significant compulsive behaviors, normative childhood rituals seem to involve emotion regulation and anxiety reduction (Evans, Gray, & Leckman, 1999). Some compulsive-like behaviors may be more likely to occur at times of transition, such as bedtime and mealtime, or when transitions are accompanied by normative fears or anxieties, such as fears of the dark (Garber et al., 1993, Werner, 1948). Rituals are frequently enacted for the purpose of warding off dreaded events or harm to the self or others, especially parents (Marks, 1987). Indeed, compulsive-like behaviors in children are positively correlated with fears and phobias (Evans et al., 1999, Zohar and Felz, 2001). That children’s compulsive behaviors may be associated with fear and anxiety is consistent with the anxiety reduction model of OCD, which has reaped great success in the treatment of OCD (e.g., Foa & Kozak, 1986).

Another approach to understanding both normal and pathological compulsive behaviors emphasizes their cognitive and neuropsychological underpinnings. OCD has been linked to deficits in executive function (Aronowitz et al., 1994, Otto, 1992, Schultz et al., 1999, Tallis, 1997). Executive function (EF) refers to a broad range of higher-order cognitive processes that coordinate information processing and behavioral control, including planning, goal-directed behavior, impulse control, working memory, sustained attention and effort, and cognitive flexibility (Heyder et al., 2004, Keil and Kaszniak, 2002). Two EF abilities that have received considerable attention in OCD research are set-shifting and response inhibition.

Set-shifting refers to the ability to learn and respond to a sorting rule, and to subsequently adapt when the rule is changed. Although some research suggests that individuals with OCD make significantly more perseverative errors than normal controls on set-shifting tasks (Aronowitz et al., 1994, Harvey, 1986, Head et al., 1989, Hymas et al., 1991, Lucey et al., 1997), other studies have failed to observe differences in set-shifting performance between normal controls and individuals with OCD (Abbruzzese et al., 1995, Boone et al., 1991, Christensen et al., 1992, Zielinski et al., 1991). In contrast to the relatively variable findings with regard to set-shifting in OCD (see Schultz et al., 1999), more consistent findings have been reported for deficits in response inhibition. Response inhibition refers to the ability to engage a behavioral response under one condition, while inhibiting a previously reinforced or prepotent response (Casbon, Curtain, Lang, & Patrick, 2003). Individuals with OCD make more errors of commission than normal controls in “go/no-go” paradigms that require inhibitory control (Abbruzzese et al., 1995, Bannon et al., 2002, Cavedini et al., 1998, Tien et al., 1992). The severity of OC symptoms has also been shown to positively correlate with response-suppression failures (Rosenberg, Dick, O’Hearn, & Sweeney, 1997).

Like clinically-relevant compulsions, normative ritualistic, compulsive-like behaviors may be associated with neuropsychological task performance. Evans, Lewis, and Iobst (2004) report that highly ritualistic and perfectionistic children performed more poorly on set-shifting and response inhibition tasks than children who exhibited fewer compulsive behaviors. Both ritualistic behaviors and executive control (such as set-shifting and response inhibition) undergo developmental changes, particularly during three and six years of age (Carver et al., 2001, Diamond and Taylor, 1996, Jones et al., 2003, Livesey and Morgan, 1991, Zelazo et al., 2004, Zelazo et al., 1996). These gains translate into behavior that is increasingly deliberate and self-regulated over the course of childhood. Critical changes in EF are thus concurrent with periods of normative ritualistic activity and have been attributed to changes within the frontal lobes, particularly prefrontal cortical areas (Casey et al., 1995, Casey et al., 2000, Diamond and Taylor, 1996, Dowsett and Livesey, 2000, Kopp, 1982).

Data linking neuropsychological findings to specific brain loci are abundant in the OCD literature. First, neuroimaging studies indicate that response inhibition (and associated error-monitoring) is governed by the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and the anterior cingulate cortex. Further, these cortical areas are richly connected (and partly comprise) the limbic system and the striatum—regions that are associated with emotional control as well as habit formation and the gating of intentional actions (Casey et al., 2000, Insel and Winslow, 1992). In contrast, set-shifting abilities are largely attributed to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and are generally considered less directly linked to the limbic and striatal systems (Berman et al., 1995, Filley et al., 1999, Milner, 1963, Rezai et al., 1993). While some neuroimaging studies have revealed abnormalities in the DLPFC (e.g., Adler et al., 2000), the preponderance of evidence suggests that the pathogenesis of OCD involves a disruption of neural mechanisms in the OFC-limbic-striatal circuit (for a review see Schultz et al., 1999). Though the present study did not include direct observation of brain function, we are intrigued by the maturational and pathogenic links between compulsive behaviors, inhibitory control and its neural substrates, and the promise that these links hold for developmental social neuroscience.

This study explored the neuropsychological and emotional underpinnings of ritualistic and compulsive behavior in typically developing young children. The overall aim of the study was to determine the nature of the relation between normative rituals, fears, and performance on EF tasks that presumably reflect frontal lobe abilities. It was hypothesized that children’s performance on neuropsychological tasks that assess response inhibition would be a more significant predictor of their compulsive, ritualistic behaviors than their performance on set-shifting tasks. While both set-shifting and response inhibition abilities were predicted to be related to the frequency and intensity of children’s rituals, response inhibition performance was expected to be more closely associated with compulsive-like behavior in children. In light of the existing literature on the development of EF, one could expect that in younger children, repetitive behaviors result from poor response inhibition and executive control, whereas for older children compulsive-like behaviors may reflect more intentional, purposeful behaviors indicating greater executive control. In addition, the magnitude of children’s fears was predicted to be positively associated with the intensity of ritualistic activity, given its apparent role in emotion regulation. If empirically demonstrated, these associations among emotion, cognitive performance, and compulsivity in typically developing children would suggest that common neuropsychological mechanisms underlie both normal and pathological rituals.

Section snippets

Participants

Initial participants were forty-four children between the ages of 48 and 96 months (M = 71.66, SD = 14.63). Twenty were male, and 24 were female. Children were placed into one of two age groups. Group 1 consisted of children less than or equal to 72 months, while Group 2 included children older than 72 months. The 72-month cut point not only represents the median split, but previous research also suggests that significant changes in EF capacity, the nature of children’s fears, and the

Results

A series of descriptive analyses was performed for all of the relevant variables (see Table 3, Table 4). Only variables that were normally distributed were included in subsequent analyses. Several difference variables were derived for the three set-shifting tasks when a child’s performance on distinct sets of trials was timed. For the DCCS, six variables were computed representing differences between times for several phases: phase 3 time–phase 2 time; 4–2; 2–1; 3–1; 4–1; and 4–3. The following

Discussion

This study extrapolated from neuropsychological findings from research on OCD to investigate cognitive and emotional factors that underlie the ritualistic and compulsive behaviors exhibited by typically developing children. Three converging pieces of evidence led to the hypothesis that normal childhood rituals may have neuropsychological and affective correlates that overlap with those of OCD. First, the growth of EF is a crucial event in childhood, particularly between the ages of three and

Acknowledgment

Portions of this paper were presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta, Georgia, April 2005.

References (65)

  • D.W. Evans et al.

    The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in normally developing compulsive-like behaviors and obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Brain and Cognition

    (2004)
  • C. Gerstadt et al.

    The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of 312–7-year-old children on a Stroop-like day-night test

    Cognition

    (1994)
  • D. Head et al.

    Deficit in cognitive shifting ability in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1989)
  • K. Heyder et al.

    Cortico-subcortical contributions to executive control

    Acta Psychologica

    (2004)
  • T.R. Insel et al.

    Neurobiology of obsessive compulsive disorder

    Psychiatric Clinics of North America

    (1992)
  • A.K. Kuelz et al.

    Neuropsychological performance in obsessive–compulsive disorder: A critical review

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2004)
  • H. Leonard et al.

    Childhood rituals: Normal development or obsessive–compulsive symptoms?

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

    (1990)
  • M.W. Otto

    Normal and abnormal information processing: A neuropsychological perspective on obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Psychiatric Clinics of North America

    (1992)
  • S. Saxena et al.

    Functional neuroimaging and the microanatomy of obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Psychiatric Clinics of North America

    (2000)
  • R.T. Schultz et al.

    Neuropsychological models of childhood obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America

    (1999)
  • P.D. Zelazo et al.

    Executive function across the lifespan

    Acta Psychologica

    (2004)
  • P.D. Zelazo et al.

    An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them

    Cognitive Development

    (1996)
  • B.R. Aronowitz et al.

    Neuropsychology of obsessive–compulsive disorder: Preliminary findings

    Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology

    (1994)
  • C.K. Berenson

    Anxiety and the developing child

    American Psychiatric Press Review of Psychiatry

    (1996)
  • K.B. Boone et al.

    Neuropsychological characteristics of nondepressed adults with obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology

    (1991)
  • T.S. Casbon et al.

    Deleterious effects of alcohol intoxication: Diminished cognitive control and its behavioral consequences

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2003)
  • A.C. Carver et al.

    Age related changes in inhibitory control as measured by stop signal task performance

    International Journal of Neuroscience

    (2001)
  • B.J. Casey et al.

    A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a Go-No-Go task

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (1997)
  • C.K. Conners

    Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II: Technical guide

    (2000)
  • C.K. Conners

    Conners’ Kiddie Continuous Performance Test: Technical guide

    (2001)
  • M. Dennis

    Frontal lobe function in childhood and adolescence: A heuristic for assessing attention regulation, executive control, and the intentional states important for social discourse

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (1991)
  • A. Diamond

    Abilities and neural mechanisms underlying AB performance

    Child Development

    (1988)
  • Cited by (41)

    • Behavioural disinhibition in frontotemporal dementia investigated within an ecological framework

      2023, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      In particular, social disinhibition seems to be more detected by our ecological approach than in previous non-ecological studies relying on questionnaires (65% to 98% according to Bang et al., 2015; and Desmarais et al., 2018). On the other hand, compulsivity emerges less in our cohort than what was previously described (71% to 80% of patients according to Miller & Llibre Guerra, 2019; Seeley, 2019). Concerning impulsivity, we identified this pathological behaviour in 48% of our sample and this is the first time that behavioural impulsivity has been reported in bvFTD.

    • The relation between normative rituals/routines and obsessive-compulsive symptoms at a young age: A systematic review

      2020, Developmental Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      Based on this model, we can formulate several recommendations for clinical practice. During the early detection and differential diagnosis procedures, clinicians should note that higher anxiety and limited neurological functioning (motor-suppression, response inhibition and set-shifting) are not only characteristic of OCD but also of normative rituals/routines, as they are underlying explanatory/etiological factors of both constructs on the continuum, and therefore may have less differentiating value (Boyer & Lienard, 2006; Evans & Maliken, 2011; Evans et al., 1999, 2001, 2004; Glenn & Nananidou, 2016; Glenn et al., 2012; Laing et al., 2009; Pietrefesa & Evans, 2007; Santesso et al., 2006; Zohar & Bruno, 1997; Zohar & Dahan, 2016; Zohar & Felz, 2001). Probably more helpful in distinguishing normal and pathological rituals is the child’s age.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text