Elsevier

Brain and Cognition

Volume 62, Issue 1, October 2006, Pages 9-16
Brain and Cognition

Age differences in fluid intelligence: Contributions of general slowing and frontal decline

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.02.006Get rights and content

Abstract

The current study examined the contributions of general slowing and frontal decline to age differences in fluid intelligence. Participants aged 20–89 years completed Block Design, Matrix Reasoning, simple reaction time, choice reaction time, Wisconsin Card Sorting, and Tower of London tasks. Age-related declines in fluid intelligence, speed of processing, and frontal function were observed. Hierarchical regression analyses showed that the processing speed and frontal function measures accounted for significant variance in fluid intelligence performance, but there was also a residual effect of age after controlling for each variable individually as well as both variables. An additional analysis showed that the variance in fluid intelligence that was attributable to processing speed was not fully shared with the variance attributable to frontal function. These findings suggest that the age-related decline in fluid intelligence is due to general slowing and frontal decline, as well as other unidentified factors.

Introduction

In a recent article, Salthouse (2004) makes several observations regarding the “what” and “when” of age-related changes in cognition. He notes that a number of aspects of cognition are detrimentally affected by aging, including processing speed, memory, and reasoning; the negative age trends are often large; and the decline often begins before age 50. At the same time, Salthouse points out that some aspects of cognition (e.g., vocabulary) remain fairly stable from the mid 50s onward. In comparison to what is known about the what and when of cognitive aging, much less is known about what Salthouse calls the “how” of cognitive aging, the mechanisms underlying the cognitive decline. The current study examines the mechanisms that may be responsible for the decline in one aspect of cognition, fluid intelligence.

Fluid intelligence generally refers to reasoning and novel problem-solving ability and is thought to be related to metacognition (Cattell, 1971, Gray et al., 2003, Sternberg, 1985). A number of studies, going back to early work by Horn and Cattell (1967), report an age-related decline in performance on fluid intelligence tasks. The existing literature offers several possible mechanisms for the age-related decline in fluid intelligence. Salthouse, 1996, Salthouse, 2001a contends that the age-related decline in a variety of cognitive abilities, including reasoning, can be accounted for by a single mechanism, generalized slowing. By this view, generalized slowing has a detrimental effect on cognitive function in two ways. The first is an inability to effectively execute the component operations involved in a task due to time limitations and the second is the inability to hold information on-line that is necessary for task completion.

Past aging research on the relationship between fluid intelligence and speed of processing supports the generalized slowing explanation (Hertzog, 1989, Schaie, 1989). In a four-year longitudinal study involving older adults, changes in processing speed strongly correlated with changes in fluid intelligence (r = .53; Zimprich & Martin, 2002). About 28% of the age-related variance in processing speed and fluid intelligence was shared variance. In a cross-sectional study, Salthouse (1991) reported that age differences in reasoning ability were significantly attenuated after controlling for processing speed: Across three studies, just 9–29% of the age-related variance in the reasoning measures remained unaccounted for after controlling for variance in perceptual comparison speed. In addition, Bors and Forrin (1995) found that the correlation between age and performance on a fluid intelligence task, Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices, was non-significant after controlling for mental speed.

A second explanation for age-related cognitive decline relates to the functioning of the frontal lobes. Prefrontal cortex theory (cf. West, 1996) states that goal-oriented functions of the prefrontal cortex (e.g., integrating information, executing complex, sequential behaviors, handling novelty, and inhibiting distracting or interfering information) are most susceptible to age effects because of the neurophysiological changes occurring in this area of the brain. Recent evidence suggests that the frontal lobes are one of the first areas of the brain to be negatively affected by aging. Research has identified decreases in frontal lobe volume (DeCarli et al., 2005, Raz et al., 1993) as well as alterations in frontal cell morphology (Buckner, 2004, Flood and Coleman, 1988, Masliah et al., 1993) with age. In addition, the largest age-related reductions in cerebral blood flow have been localized to the frontal and prefrontal areas (Gur et al., 1987, Mathew et al., 1986, Schroeter et al., 2003, Shaw et al., 1984).

A variety of studies suggest that the frontal lobes are recruited when performing fluid intelligence tasks. Isingrini and Vazou (1997) found, for example, that performance on traditional frontal tasks correlated with measures of fluid intelligence but not crystallized intelligence in a group of older adults. Parkin and Java (1999) showed that fluid intelligence performance accounted for 15–24% of the variance in performance on three tasks of frontal function. Moreover, in a recent functional magnetic resonance imaging study, Gray et al. (2003) found that participants who scored high on a standard measure of fluid intelligence also showed a significantly stronger blood flow response in regions of the frontal cortex (including the lateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate) during a working memory task. This pattern was especially apparent on working memory trials in which interference was high and attentional control was presumably needed to focus on goal-relevant information.

Some existing research supports a frontal explanation for the decline in fluid intelligence with age. Schretlen et al. (2000) hypothesized that “age-related atrophic changes in frontal brain structures undermine the functioning of executive abilities, and that this results in the gradual decline of fluid intelligence” (p. 53). Consistent with this view, they found that executive ability and frontal-lobe volume, but not non-frontal volume, significantly reduced the age-related variance in fluid intelligence. Note, however, that processing speed also reduced the age-related variance in fluid intelligence. Moreover, the contribution of executive ability and frontal volume to the decline in fluid intelligence was largely unrelated to the contribution of processing speed. The Schretlen et al. study therefore suggests that reduced frontal functioning and reduced processing speed both contribute to the decline in fluid intelligence with age.

Schretlen et al.’s (2000) study, as described above, is rather uncommon in that it examined both the generalized slowing and frontal explanations of cognitive aging in the same study. Parkin and Java (2000) note that studies of frontal function often do not include tests of speed or fluid intelligence, fluid intelligence studies often do not include tests of speed or frontal function, and speed of processing studies often do not include tests of frontal function or fluid intelligence. As such, most existing studies operate on a confirmatory bias rather than pitting two theories against one another. Here we follow Schretlen et al.’s lead and examine the role of both generalized slowing and frontal function in the age-related decline of fluid intelligence.

Section snippets

Overview of the study

The primary goal of the present study was to examine the contribution of age and processing speed to performance on two fluid intelligence tasks, Block Design and Matrix Reasoning. Following Salthouse, 2001b, Salthouse, 1996, we examined the residual age-related variance in fluid intelligence after statistically controlling for variance on the processing speed measures (simple reaction time, choice reaction time, and processing speed composite). Specifically, we were interested in contrasting

Participants

There were 196 participants in the study, ranging in age from 20 to 89 years old. Younger adults were recruited from the student population at the University of Colorado at Colorado Springs and received class credit for their participation or for recruiting older relatives to participate. Older adults were also recruited from local senior citizen organizations in Colorado Springs, Colorado. Volunteers 60 years of age or older received $10 for participation. All participants completed a

Fluid intelligence and speed of processing

For all analyses reported in this study, the α level was set at .05. Linear regression analyses revealed significant age-related declines in performance on the Block Design task [F (1, 195) = 66.26, p < .001, R2 = .26, M = 42.3, SD = 15.1] and Matrix Reasoning task [F (1, 195) = 101.47, p < .001, R2 = .34, M = 26.2, SD = 5.8]. There were also age-related increases in simple reaction time [F (1, 194) = 26.66, p < .001, R2 = .12, M = 397, SD = 142], and choice reaction time [F (1, 194) = 68.43, p < .001, R2 = .26, M = 497, SD = 91]. Accuracy

Discussion

The primary purpose of the current study was to examine existing hypotheses regarding the decline in fluid intelligence in older adulthood. Following Parkin and Java’s (2000) suggestion, we included measures of processing speed, frontal function, and fluid intelligence in the same study. As observed in past studies, we found an age-related decline in fluid intelligence as measured by Block Design and Matrix Reasoning tasks. Hierarchical regression analyses were then used to test the generalized

References (46)

  • Bugg, J.M., DeLosh, E.L., Davalos, D.B., Davis, H.P. (in press). Age differences in Stroop interference: Contributions...
  • R.B. Cattell

    Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action

    (1971)
  • A. Dagher et al.

    Mapping the network for planning: A correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task

    Brain

    (1999)
  • Davis, H.P., Keller, F.R., 1998. Colorado assessment test manual. Colorado Springs: Colorado Assessment...
  • C.F. Emery et al.

    Longitudinal and genetic effects in the relationship between pulmonary function and cognitive performance

    Journals of Gerontology Series B-Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences.

    (1998)
  • J.R. Gray et al.

    Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2003)
  • R.C. Gur et al.

    Age and regional cerebral blood flow at rest and during cognitive activity

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1987)
  • R.K. Heaton et al.

    Wisconsin card sorting test manual: Revised

    (1993)
  • C. Hertzog

    Influences of cognitive slowing on age differences in intelligence

    Developmental Psychology

    (1989)
  • C. Hertzog et al.

    Age-related slowing in semantic information processing speed: An individual differences analysis

    Journal of Gerontology

    (1986)
  • M. Isingrini et al.

    Relation between fluid intelligence and frontal lobe in older adults

    International Journal of Aging and Human Development

    (1997)
  • M.J. Kane et al.

    The role of the prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective

    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

    (2002)
  • S.T. Kwong See et al.

    Cognitive mediation of adult age differences in language performance

    Psychology and Aging

    (1995)
  • Cited by (149)

    • Link between fluid/crystallized intelligence and global/local visual abilities across adulthood

      2022, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Fluid intelligence represents the composite performance score on tasks of memory, executive function, and processing speed; crystallized intelligence represents the composite performance score on tasks on picture vocabulary and oral reading recognition. Fluid and crystallized intelligence scores were therefore considered as complementary measures of cognition as: (1) they are both important in everyday functioning but are differently affected by age (Akshoomoff et al., 2013; Bugg, Zook, DeLosh, Davalos, & Davis, 2006; Harada et al., 2013; Heaton et al., 2014; Horn & Cattell, 1967), and (2) have been shown to be more reliable than the variables extracted from the subtests (Ott et al., 2022; Taylor et al., 2022). The t-scores (i.e., corrected for age, sex, ethnicity/race, and education) of each variable were used for further analyses.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text