Elsevier

Aggression and Violent Behavior

Volume 45, March–April 2019, Pages 51-74
Aggression and Violent Behavior

Standing up to bullying: A social ecological review of peer defending in offline and online contexts

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2018.05.007Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Both offline and online defending are associated with individual, peer, family, and school correlates.

  • Empathy was the most researched individual correlate, with all significant findings (75%) positively related to defending.

  • For offline defending, sociometric status was the most researched correlate, and its association may be bidirectional.

  • The bystander model was the most studied correlate of online defending, with most research (84%) noting a positive link.

Abstract

Bullying is a relationship problem that most often occurs in the presence of peers. Peers who witness bullying play a critical role in intervening. Peer intervention, or defending, is a complex behavior. Defending a victimized peer can occur offline and online, with many similarities between the two contexts. This paper, guided by the Social Ecological Model, systematically reviewed the correlates associated with defending at different levels including: individual, peer, family, and school. Inclusion criteria retrieved a final sample of 130 original, peer-reviewed research articles on offline defending, and 25 articles for online defending.

Consistent results across both contexts reveal that individuals who defend tend to be girls, have high empathy and low moral disengagement, are popular and well-liked by their peers, and perceive supportive relationships with their parents, teachers, and schools. More research is needed to understand interactions that may occur between levels of the model, as defending is a complex behavior that cannot be characterized by isolated correlates.

Introduction

Bullying is a relationship problem that most often occurs in the presence of other youth. Peers play an important role in protecting one another against bullying. Youth who intervene to support their bullied peers are known as “defenders”. Defending represents any prosocial action taken to stop bullying, including actively intervening to stop the bullying scenario, seeking support from an adult, and comforting the victimized youth (Salmivalli, 2010). These prosocial behaviors can happen in both offline and online contexts. The majority of peers who witness bullying do not intervene, with only 17–19% of witnesses taking the role of a defender in offline bullying (Hawkins, Pepler, & Craig, 2001; Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Bjorkqvist, Österman, & Kaukiainen, 1996). For online defending, prevalence rates vary between 9 and 50% (Dillon & Bushman, 2015; Freis & Gurung, 2013; Huang & Chou, 2010; Lenhart et al., 2011). When peers do defend, however, they often are effective in stopping bullying (Hawkins et al., 2001). In addition, peer defending behavior is negatively associated with bullying frequency within classrooms (Salmivalli, Voeten, & Poskiparta, 2011). Students who are victimized directly benefit from peer defending. They report higher self-esteem, peer acceptance, and popularity relative to their non-defended peers (Sainio, Veenstra, Huitsing, & Salmivalli, 2010). Despite the potential benefits of defending, many students remain passive bystanders when they witness bullying. Defending is a complex social interaction that unfolds in multiple social contexts. The current study employed a Social Ecological Model to guide a systematic review of the individual, family, peer, and school correlates of defending behavior in school bullying.

Social Ecological Theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1979) conceptualizes human development as the result of many nested social contexts. Individual factors, such as gender and personality, are at the innermost level, followed by the peers, families, schools, neighborhoods, and societal factors at the most distal level. Each level of influence is theorized to both directly influence individual development, and to interact with more proximal factors (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). The Social Ecological Model has been extensively examined in youth development and is readily applicable to the study of bullying and peer defending because these are complex relationship processes that are influenced by the social context. Empirical research also highlights the necessity of adopting a Social Ecological framework for conceptualizing defending behavior. Rates of defending can be highly variable between peer groups, classrooms, and schools. For example, research demonstrates that 36% of the variance within defending can be attributed to differences between peer groups (Espelage, Green, & Polanin, 2012). Similarly, estimates suggest that approximately 10–36% of the variance within defending can be attributed to differences between classrooms (Peets, Pöyhönen, Juvonen, & Salmivalli, 2015; Pozzoli, Gini, & Vieno, 2012; Salmivalli et al., 2011). Thus, an approach that takes these multiple levels into account may help us to best understand the factors that promote defending behavior, as defending is more than a product of individual differences.

Offline and online defending differ in a few key ways. First, the timing of defending differs across contexts. Offline, opportunities to defend occur in the same moment as the bullying takes place, whereas for cyberbullying, the opportunities for defending are ongoing. For instance, mean comments or embarrassing photos can be redistributed or replicated at any time without a defined end point online. Due to the continuous and permanent nature of cyberbullying, it allows individuals to defend online at any point in time, rather than only when the cyberbullying initially takes place. Second, the level of risk or costs associated with defending may vary across these two contexts, making offline defending potentially more risky than online defending. Online defenders may be anonymous or physically removed from the situation while offline defenders are not. Third, the method by which support is given may differ across contexts. Online defenders may comfort victimized individuals in both offline and online environments (i.e., by reaching out at school or on the Internet through private messages or in a public forum), while offline defending most often occurs in person (Machackova, Dedkova, Sevcikova, & Cerna, 2016). While offline and online defending are unique behaviors, they are likely influenced by the same levels of the Social Ecological Model due to the high overlap in offline and online bullying. Youth who experience cyberbullying are often victimized by the same individuals both offline and online, and are more likely to be cyberbullied by students in their same class than by students in different classes (Wegge, Vandebosch, & Eggermont, 2014). In addition, youth are more likely to defend schoolmates who experience cyberbullying than strangers, suggesting that offline peer group and classroom contexts matter in online defending (Machackova et al., 2016). Given the overlap in offline and online defending, the social context may play a large role in influencing both offline and online defending behavior. In this paper, we review the research from a Social Ecological perspective on both offline and online defending.

Section snippets

Inclusion criteria

Articles were included in the review if a) they included measures of bullying/victimization and defending, b) the sample consisted of school-aged youth and/or university populations, c) they were written in English, and d) they were empirical articles (i.e., not reviews or meta-analyses) published in peer-reviewed journals.

Search strategy

First, relevant databases (i.e., PSYCInfo, ERIC, and PubMed; Google Scholar was used to find full-text articles when necessary) were searched to identify articles that met

Descriptives

Table 1 outlines descriptive information for included studies. One hundred thirty1 articles met inclusion criteria for the review on offline defending, and 25 articles met inclusion criteria for online defending. The majority of studies

Discussion

Bullying is a group phenomenon that most often occurs in the presence of bystanders, some of whom intervene to defend the victimized youth. The literature on defending behavior in school bullying has been expanding over the last several years. In particular, research has focused primarily on the individual correlates that distinguish defenders from youth in other bullying participant roles. The current study was guided by the Social Ecological Model in reviewing studies examining the correlates

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the Bully Lab for their support in the writing of this review paper.

Funding sources

Laura J. Lambe was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) Vanier Canada Scholarship. Victoria Della Cioppa and Irene K. Hong were supported by a SSHRC Joseph-Armand Bombardier Canada Graduate Scholarship. Wendy Craig was supported by grants from the National Centres of Excellence and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council.

References2 (167)

  • G. *Gini et al.

    Determinants of adolescents' active defending and passive bystanding behavior in bullying

    Journal of Adolescence

    (2008)
  • G. *Gini et al.

    Bullies have enhanced moral competence to judge relative to victims, but lack moral compassion

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2011)
  • Y.-Y. Huang et al.

    An analysis of multiple factors of cyberbullying among junior high school students in Taiwan

    Computers in Human Behavior

    (2010)
  • T. *Jungert et al.

    Early adolescents' motivations to defend victims in school bullying and their perceptions of student-teacher relationships: A self- determination theory approach

    Journal of Adolescence

    (2016)
  • L.J. *Lambe et al.

    Does defending come with a cost? Examining the psychosocial correlates of defending behaviour among bystanders of bullying in a Canadian sample

    Child Abuse & Neglect

    (2017)
  • Y. *Li et al.

    Roles of fatalism and parental support in the relationship between bullying victimization and bystander behaviors

    School Psychology International

    (2015)
  • H. *Machackova et al.

    Brief report: The bystander effect in cyberbullying incidents

    Journal of Adolescence

    (2015)
  • D.J. Meter et al.

    Defenders of victims of peer aggression: Interdependence theory and an exploration of individual, interpersonal, and contextual effects on the defender participant role

    Developmental Review

    (2015)
  • A.B. *Nickerson et al.

    Attachment and empathy as predictors of roles as defenders or outsiders in bullying interactions

    Journal of School Psychology

    (2008)
  • P. *O'Connell et al.

    Peer involvement in bullying: Insights and challenges for intervention

    Journal of Adolescence

    (1999)
  • B. *Oldenburg et al.

    Peer and self-reported victimization: Do non-victimized students give victimization nominations to classmates who are self-reported victims?

    Journal of School Psychology

    (2015)
  • L.J. *Patterson et al.

    Adolescent bystanders' perspectives of aggression in the online versus school environments

    Journal of Adolescence

    (2016)
  • E. *Ahmed

    Stop it, that's enough: Bystander intervention and its relationship to school connectedness and shame management

    Vulnerable Children and Youth Studies

    (2008)
  • A. *Almeida et al.

    Moral disengagement, normative beliefs of peer group, and attitudes regarding roles in bullying

    Journal of School Violence

    (2010)
  • E. *Andreou et al.

    The relationship of academic and social cognition to behaviour in bullying situations among Greek primary school children

    Educational Psychology

    (2004)
  • K. *Barchia et al.

    Predictors of student defenders of peer aggression victims: Empathy and social cognitive factors

    International Journal of Behavioral Development

    (2011)
  • L.R. *Barhight et al.

    Relations between actual group norms, perceived peer behavior, and bystander children's intervention to bullying

    Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology

    (2017)
  • L.R. *Barhight et al.

    Children's physiological and emotional reactions to witnessing bullying predict bystander intervention

    Child Development

    (2013)
  • S. *Bastiaensens et al.

    “Can I afford to help?” How affordances of communication modalities guide bystanders' helping intentions towards harassment on social network sites

    Behaviour & Information Technology

    (2015)
  • S. *Begeer et al.

    Bullying-related behaviour in a mainstream high school versus a high school for autism: Self-report and peer-report

    Autism

    (2016)
  • C. *Belacchi et al.

    Prosocial/hostile roles and emotion comprehension in preschoolers

    Aggressive Behavior

    (2010)
  • N. *Brody et al.

    Bystander intervention in cyberbullying

    Communication Monographs

    (2016)
  • U. Bronfenbrenner

    Contexts of child rearing: Problems and prospects

    American Psychologist

    (1979)
  • M. *Camodeca et al.

    Bullying in preschool: The associations between participant roles, social competence, and social preference

    Aggressive Behavior

    (2015)
  • M. *Camodeca et al.

    Bullying, empathic concern, and internalization of rules among preschool children: The role of emotion understanding

    International Journal of Behavioral Development

    (2016)
  • M. *Camodeca et al.

    Aggression, social cognitions, anger and sadness in bullies and victims

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • M. *Camodeca et al.

    Children's opinions on effective strategies to cope with bullying: The importance of bullying role and perspective

    Educational Research

    (2005)
  • M.C. *Cappadocia et al.

    Individual motivations and characteristics associated with bystander intervention during bullying episodes among children and youth

    Canadian Journal of School Psychology

    (2012)
  • S. *Caravita et al.

    Early adolescents' participation in bullying: Is ToM involved?

    The Journal of Early Adolescence

    (2010)
  • S.C.S. *Caravita et al.

    Unique and interactive effects of empathy and social status on involvement in bullying

    Social Development

    (2009)
  • S.C.S. *Caravita et al.

    Main and moderated effects of moral cognition and status on bullying and defending

    Aggressive Behavior

    (2012)
  • E.A. *Casey et al.

    The situational-cognitive model of adolescent bystander behavior: Modeling bystander decision-making in the context of bullying and teen dating violence

    Psychology of Violence

    (2017)
  • L.-M. *Chen et al.

    Choosing to be a defender or an outsider in a school bullying incident: Determining factors and the defending process

    School Psychology International

    (2016)
  • S. *Choi et al.

    Influence of psychological and social factors on bystanders' roles in school bullying among Korean-American students in the United States

    School Psychology International

    (2012)
  • I. *Correia et al.

    School bullying: Belief in a personal just world of bullies, victims, and defenders

    European Psychologist

    (2008)
  • I. *Correia et al.

    Belief in a just world and well-being of bullies, victims and defenders: A study with Portuguese and Indian students

    Anxiety, Stress, and Coping

    (2009)
  • S.M. *Coyne et al.

    Pow! Boom! Kablam! Effects of viewing superhero programs on aggressive, prosocial, and defending behaviors in preschool children

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2017)
  • W. Craig et al.

    A cross-national profile of bullying and victimization among adolescents in 40 countries

    International Journal of Public Health

    (2009)
  • A.M. *Crapanzano et al.

    Gender differences in the assessment, stability, and correlates to bullying roles in middle school children

    Behavioral Sciences & the Law

    (2011)
  • J.M. Darley et al.

    Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1968)
  • Cited by (0)

    2

    References marked with an asterisk (*) indicate studies included in the review.

    View full text