Elsevier

Ageing Research Reviews

Volume 27, May 2016, Pages 1-14
Ageing Research Reviews

Review
Walking ability to predict future cognitive decline in old adults: A scoping review

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2016.02.001Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Accuracy of prediction models for cognitive decline could be improved.

  • Motor and cognitive functions share neuroanatomical structures and psychological processes.

  • We review longitudinal data on the relation between walking ability and future change in cognition.

  • Gait slowing precedes decline in cognitive functions and dementia syndromes.

  • Including dynamic gait measures could strengthen prediction models for cognitive decline.

Abstract

Early identification of individuals at risk for cognitive decline may facilitate the selection of those who benefit most from interventions. Current models predicting cognitive decline include neuropsychological and/or biological markers. Additional markers based on walking ability might improve accuracy and specificity of these models because motor and cognitive functions share neuroanatomical structures and psychological processes. We reviewed the relationship between walking ability at one point of (mid) life and cognitive decline at follow-up. A systematic literature search identified 20 longitudinal studies. The average follow-up time was 4.5 years. Gait speed quantified walking ability in most studies (n = 18). Additional gait measures (n = 4) were step frequency, variability and step-length. Despite methodological weaknesses, results revealed that gait slowing (0.68–1.1 m/sec) preceded cognitive decline and the presence of dementia syndromes (maximal odds and hazard ratios of 10.4 and 11.1, respectively). The results indicate that measures of walking ability could serve as additional markers to predict cognitive decline. However, gait speed alone might lack specificity. We recommend gait analysis, including dynamic gait parameters, in clinical evaluations of patients with suspected cognitive decline. Future studies should focus on examining the specificity and accuracy of various gait characteristics to predict future cognitive decline.

Introduction

The increase in the number of old adults nearly parallels the incidence of age-associated dementia worldwide (Ferri et al., 2005, Prince et al., 2013). Data suggest that the pathophysiological processes of dementia may start several years or even decades before the eventual diagnosis (Sperling et al., 2013, Morris et al., 2012). Patients progress from a preclinical phase during which the disease might have already started in the brain without overt clinical symptoms, followed by a period characterized by the presence of Mild Cognitive Impairments (MCI), culminating in a diagnosis of dementia (DeCarli et al., 2012). In the absence of a cure, key strategies of disease management include early diagnosis, delaying disease onset, and a slowing of disease progression (de la Torre, 2010, Imtiaz et al., 2014). Therefore, identifying markers that predict dementia is a major subject of current interest (Li and Zhang, 2015, Panza et al., 2015).

Prediction of dementia is often studied in the context of MCI (Petersen et al., 2001), which is a transitional state between a cognitively intact condition and dementia (DeCarli, 2003). Patients with MCI have cognitive dysfunctions beyond those expected as a result of normal aging, yet the level of impairment is not severe enough to compromise the ability to perform activities of daily living (Petersen et al., 1999). Even though the published values vary, a recent review analyzing population data (>300 participants) estimated the prevalence of MCI to range from 16 to 20% in patients over age 60. Approximately 10 to 15% of these patients develop dementia annually (Roberts, 2013). This conversion rate is high, making it important to differentiate between patients who will develop dementia and those who will remain cognitively fit. Early identification of patients at risk for dementia might help to select those individuals who would benefit most from future interventions to delay disease onset and slow the progression of neurodegeneration (Friedrich, 2014).

Biomarkers are used to identify pre-dementia symptoms and can be broadly classified as (1) cognitive markers (test scores measuring cognitive functioning such as memory and executive function) and (2) biological markers (such as measures derived from cerebrospinal fluid and brain imaging). The most accurate predictors are memory tasks measuring long-delay free recall (Gomar et al., 2011, Schmand et al., 2010, Landau et al., 2010, Fleisher et al., 2008, Gallagher et al., 2010), the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) markers Aβ1—42/t-tau ratio (Gomar et al., 2011, Vos et al., 2012, Hansson et al., 2006, Mattsson et al., 2009), and volumes of the hippocampal and entorhinal cortices (Gomar et al., 2011, Devanand et al., 2007, Vos et al., 2012, Prestia et al., 2013, Ewers et al., 2012). However, single predictors seem to be insufficiently sensitive to predict conversion from MCI to dementia. Therefore, prediction models ultimately employ a combination of markers (Shaffer et al., 2013). Nevertheless, such predictions are far from perfect, as age, duration of follow-up, subtype of MCI diagnosis, degree of cognitive decline (early versus late stage of MCI), and outcome (e.g., AD, mixed dementia) all seem to affect conversion rates (Schmand et al., 2010, Egli et al., 2015, Espinosa et al., 2013). For example, a recent study showed that both neuropsychological assessment and MRI variables can predict conversion to AD with 63–67% classification accuracy in patients with MCI both younger and older than 75, while CSF biomarkers reached this rate only in patients younger than 75 years old (Schmand et al., 2010). A systematic review about risk prediction models for dementia concluded that sensitivity and specificity values vary broadly between studies, (Area Under the Curve ranging from AUC = 50 to AUC = 87). In particular, specificity is low in numerous prediction models (Stephan et al., 2010), complicating the clinical use of such models.

Taken together, these observations show that it remains a persistent challenge and should be a research priority to develop dementia prediction models that ultimately employ a combination of markers to differentiate between old adults who will and who will not develop dementia. Current prediction models show low to moderate predictive ability with large variability, making it necessary to explore new markers. A possible candidate is motor function, in which walking ability may serve as a potential marker in the prediction of cognitive decline (Ambrose et al., 2010, Montero-Odasso et al., 2012b, Verghese et al., 2002).

The original observation of a correlation between motor and cognitive impairments was reported nearly two decades ago. The data suggested that motor slowing (e.g., low walking speed) precedes cognitive decline in healthy older adults (Camicioli et al., 1998), a finding substantiated by the relationship between reductions in gait function and the development of dementia (Richards et al., 1993). Numerous cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have recently confirmed these initial findings (Callisaya et al., 2015, Gale et al., 2014, Ijmker and Lamoth, 2012, Scherder et al., 2007, Verghese et al., 2013).

Viewing walking as a complex task could increase its validity to serve as a marker for early cognitive decline. Indeed, imaging and brain stimulation studies suggest that higher brain centers are involved in the planning and execution of normal human locomotion (Christensen et al., 1998) and balance (Taube et al., 2015, Papegaaij et al., 2014). The widespread network of brain areas that control walking involves regions responsible for attentional, executive and visuospatial functions as well as areas needed to perform and control motor tasks, such as the cerebellum, basal ganglia and motor cortex (Holtzer et al., 2014). Thus, there is an overlap between areas that control walking and areas that control cognitive functioning, explaining the relationship between dementia-related pathology and gait dysfunction. The co-occurrence of decline in both cognitive and gait function favors a ‘common-cause’ mechanism (Christensen et al., 2001). There is considerable evidence for the role of white matter damage in age-related cognitive decline and dementia (Debette and Markus, 2010, Jokinen et al., 2012). In addition, reduced gray and white matter volumes in multiple brain regions and white matter hyperintensities are associated with gait dysfunction (gait speed of <0.5 m/s) in old adults free from dementia (Callisaya et al., 2013).

Perhaps the simplest demonstration of the interrelationship between gait and cognition comes from dual task studies, in which subjects perform a walking and cognitively demanding task concurrently (Lundin-Olsson et al., 1997). ‘Dual task cost’, i.e., the magnitude of deterioration in gait performance measured during single vs. dual tasking, arises from the two interfering tasks competing for the same cortical resources (Camicioli et al., 1997). It is noteworthy that dual task costs are often higher in cognitively impaired compared to cognitively intact elderly (Camicioli et al., 1997, Sheridan et al., 2003, Montero-Odasso et al., 2012a, Lamoth et al., 2011).

The effects of decline in cognition on walking are especially expressed in the slowing of gait. A ubiquitous observation from cross-sectional studies is the reduction of gait speed in patients with MCI (Montero-Odasso et al., 2014, Tseng et al., 2014, Nascimbeni et al., 2012) and dementia (Verghese et al., 2013, van Iersel et al., 2004, Ijmker and Lamoth, 2012, Lamoth et al., 2011). In addition to gait speed, spatial variability and stride time variability (STV) tend to increase in patients with MCI (Boripuntakul et al., 2014, Beauchet et al., 2014). However, for the time being, most studies have cross-sectional designs and are restricted to gait speed as a measure of walking ability.

The co-occurrence of gait dysfunction and decline in cognitive function as derived from cross-sectional studies suggests that measures of walking ability could serve as a marker in the identification of individuals at risk to develop dementia. To verify the possibility that gait dysfunction precedes cognitive decline, we set the aim of the present review to scope evidence from longitudinal studies that assessed whether or not there is a relationship between walking ability at one point of (mid) life and cognitive decline years later. In addition, we critically evaluate and discuss methodologies used to determine this relationship and to formulate recommendations for future studies to expand the preclinical phase of dementia.

Section snippets

Scoping review

A scoping review method was adopted to explore the depth of evidence for the putative role of walking ability in the prediction of cognitive decline. A scoping review provides an appropriate method to systematically scan and evaluate evidence within a specific area of research and to identify gaps in the existing literature, allowing variation in methods between studies selected for inclusion (Armstrong et al., 2011, Levac et al., 2010).

Literature search

A systematic literature search was performed for studies

Literature search

The literature search revealed 431 studies of which after screening for title and abstract, 50 were assessed for eligibility by full-text analysis. Finally, 20 articles met the criteria for inclusion. A flowchart of the literature search and selection process is presented in Fig. 1.

Study characteristics

Studies included in the current review were heterogeneous in terms of number of participants (ranging from 52 to 2776), age (>60 – >80) and length of follow-up (ranging from 2 to 9 years) and are based on data from

Discussion

The present scoping review aimed to examine the relationship between walking ability and future cognitive decline. The main finding supported the hypothesis that walking ability at baseline, independent of gait characteristic, has the potential to predict future cognitive decline through (1) an association between poor walking ability at baseline and within-person decline in cognition at follow-up and (2) a higher risk for cognitive impairment/dementia with poor walking ability at baseline as

Conclusions

It is a health priority to improve dementia prediction models. The present scoping review aimed to determine the relationship between walking ability at baseline and future cognitive state. The results emerging from 20 studies demonstrated that gait slowing preceded cognitive decline in mental state, specific cognitive functions and dementia syndromes, and support the hypothesis that measures of walking ability could serve as a marker in the prediction of cognitive decline. Therefore we

References (101)

  • E. Hogervorst et al.

    Low free testosterone is an independent risk factor for Alzheimer's disease

    Exp. Gerontol.

    (2004)
  • J.H. Hollman et al.

    Age-related differences in spatiotemporal markers of gait stability during dual task walking

    Gait Posture

    (2007)
  • T. Ijmker et al.

    Gait and cognition: the relationship between gait stability and variability with executive function in persons with and without dementia

    Gait Posture

    (2012)
  • B. Imtiaz et al.

    Future directions in Alzheimer's disease from risk factors to prevention

    Biochem. Pharmacol.

    (2014)
  • U. Lindemann et al.

    Distance to achieve steady state walking speed in frail elderly persons

    Gait Posture

    (2008)
  • L. Lundin-Olsson et al.

    Stops walking when talking as a predictor of falls in elderly people

    Lancet

    (1997)
  • M. Montero-Odasso et al.

    Dual-task complexity affects gait in people with mild cognitive impairment: the interplay between gait variability dual tasking, and risk of falls

    Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil.

    (2012)
  • A. Ojagbemi et al.

    Gait speed and cognitive decline over 2 years in the Ibadan study of aging

    Gait Posture

    (2015)
  • A. Prestia et al.

    Translational outpatient memory clinic working group, Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging initiative, 2013. Diagnostic accuracy of markers for prodromal Alzheimer's disease in independent clinical series

    Alzheimers Dement.

    (2013)
  • R. Roberts

    Epidemiology of mild cognitive impairment: the mayo clinic study of aging

    Alzheimer's Dement.

    (2013)
  • E. Scherder et al.

    Gait in ageing and associated dementias; its relationship with cognition

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2007)
  • P. Szulc et al.

    Hormonal and lifestyle determinants of appendicular skeletal muscle mass in men: the MINOS study

    Am. J. Clin. Nutr.

    (2004)
  • W. Taube et al.

    Brain activity during observation and motor imagery of different balance tasks: an fMRI study

    Cortex

    (2015)
  • S. Vos et al.

    Test sequence of CSF and MRI biomarkers for prediction of AD in subjects with MCI

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2012)
  • L.M. Waite et al.

    Gait slowing as a predictor of incident dementia: 6-year longitudinal data from the Sydney older persons study

    J. Neurol. Sci.

    (2005)
  • R.D. Abbott et al.

    Walking and dementia in physically capable elderly men

    JAMA

    (2004)
  • G. Abellan van Kan et al.

    Gait speed body composition, and dementia. The EPIDOS-Toulouse cohort

    J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci.

    (2012)
  • A. Alfaro-Acha et al.

    Does 8-foot walk time predict cognitive decline in older Mexicans Americans?

    J. Am. Geriatr. Soc.

    (2007)
  • A.F. Ambrose et al.

    Gait and cognition in older adults: Insights from the Bronx and Kerala

    Ann. Indian Acad. Neurol.

    (2010)
  • R. Armstrong et al.

    Cochrane update: ‘scoping the scope' of a cochrane review

    J. Public Health (Oxf.)

    (2011)
  • O. Beauchet et al.

    Motor phenotype of decline in cognitive performance among community-dwellers without dementia: population-based study and meta-analysis

    PLoS One

    (2014)
  • J.S. Brach et al.

    Validation of a measure of smoothness of walking

    J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci.

    (2010)
  • S.A. Bridenbaugh et al.

    Quantitative gait disturbances in older adults with cognitive impairments

    Curr. Pharm. Des.

    (2014)
  • T. Buracchio et al.

    The trajectory of gait speed preceding mild cognitive impairment

    Arch. Neurol.

    (2010)
  • M.L. Callisaya et al.

    Brain structural change and gait decline: a longitudinal population-based study

    J. Am. Geriatr. Soc.

    (2013)
  • M.L. Callisaya et al.

    Longitudinal relationships between cognitive decline and gait slowing: the tasmanian study of cognition and gait

    J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci.

    (2015)
  • R. Camicioli et al.

    Talking while walking: the effect of a dual task in aging and Alzheimer's disease

    Neurology

    (1997)
  • R. Camicioli et al.

    Motor slowing precedes cognitive impairment in the oldest old

    Neurology

    (1998)
  • Castell, M.V., Sanchez, M., Julian, R., Queipo, R., Martin, S., Otero, A., 2013. Frailty prevalence and slow walking...
  • M. Cesari et al.

    Inflammatory markers and physical performance in older persons: the InCHIANTI study

    J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci.

    (2004)
  • H. Christensen et al.

    The common cause hypothesis of cognitive aging: evidence for not only a common factor but also specific associations of age with vision and grip strength in a cross-sectional analysis

    Psychol. Aging

    (2001)
  • L.O. Christensen et al.

    Corticospinal function during human walking

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (1998)
  • M.L. Daviglus et al.

    National Institutes of Health State-of-the-Science Conference statement: preventing alzheimer disease and cognitive decline

    Ann. Intern. Med.

    (2010)
  • J.C. de la Torre

    Alzheimer's disease is incurable but preventable

    J. Alzheimers Dis.

    (2010)
  • S. Debette et al.

    The clinical importance of white matter hyperintensities on brain magnetic resonance imaging: systematic review and meta-analysis

    BMJ

    (2010)
  • C. DeCarli et al.

    Session II: mechanisms of age-related cognitive change and targets for intervention: neural circuits, networks, and plasticity

    J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci.

    (2012)
  • K. Deckers et al.

    Target risk factors for dementia prevention: a systematic review and Delphi consensus study on the evidence from observational studies

    Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry

    (2015)
  • N. Deshpande et al.

    Gait speed under varied challenges and cognitive decline in older persons: a prospective study

    Age Ageing

    (2009)
  • D.P. Devanand et al.

    Hippocampal and entorhinal atrophy in mild cognitive impairment: prediction of Alzheimer disease

    Neurology

    (2007)
  • S.C. Egli et al.

    Varying strength of cognitive markers and biomarkers to predict conversion and cognitive decline in an early-stage-enriched mild cognitive impairment sample

    J. Alzheimers Dis.

    (2015)
  • Cited by (119)

    • The effects of aerobic exercise and transcranial direct current stimulation on cognitive function in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis

      2022, Ageing Research Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Notwithstanding repeated calls and conceptual frameworks, our systematic search identified little to no data concerning the synergistic effects of pairing chronic AE with chronic tDCS on CG, WM, and EF in the three populations. Synergistic effects between concurrent AE and tDCS could arise because cognition and walking and running, activities often used in AE and rehabilitation, share common neural substrates (Dougherty et al., 2021; Kikkert et al., 2016; Moreau et al., 2015; Morris et al., 2016; Steinberg et al., 2019; Verlinden et al., 2014). In particular, the frontal lobe, where circuits controlling EF reside, becomes increasingly activated as the speed and complexity of walking increase (Clark et al., 2014; Wagshul et al., 2019).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text