Elsevier

Appetite

Volume 67, 1 August 2013, Pages 81-87
Appetite

Research report
Emotional, external and restrained eating behaviour and BMI trajectories in adolescence

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.03.014Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We model trajectories of adolescents’ BMI development with growth mixture modelling.

  • Development of BMI was best fitted in five distinct trajectories with similar moderate BMI increase.

  • Restrained eaters had a higher change of being in the higher BMI trajectories.

  • Emotional and external eating were unrelated to BMI trajectory membership.

Abstract

Individual differences in eating behaviours might partly explain the variations in development of weight gain and subsequent overweight and obesity. In the current study, identified trajectories of BMI in adolescence and their associations with restrained, emotional and external eating were tested. For the assessment of BMI trajectories growth mixture modelling was used; a method used to identify clusters of individuals within a population that follow distinct developmental trajectories. In total 328 Dutch adolescents (13–15 years old at baseline) self-reported their height and weight at five annual waves and their eating behaviour at baseline. Development of BMI was best fitted in five distinct trajectories that showed similar moderate increase of BMI over time; parallel but at a different level. High restrained eaters had a higher chance of being in the higher BMI trajectories. Emotional and external eating were unrelated to the BMI trajectories. In conclusion, adolescents in this study followed very parallel patterns of moderate increases in BMI which suggests that factors acting on individual differences in weight status have had their influence mostly at a – perhaps much – younger age. Restraint eating was related to BMI in early adolescence, but not to an increases or decreases in BMI over the course of adolescence.

Introduction

The literature on obesity and eating pathology in adolescents has increasingly focused on both environment and individual differences in eating behaviours and physical exercise. Even in times where the prevalence of childhood obesity has increased profoundly in most Western countries (Wang & Lobstein, 2006), the question remains how we can explain that some youths keep their weight in balance for years, while others become overweight. Individual differences in eating behaviours might partly explain the variations in development of weight gain and subsequent obesity (van Strien, Frijters, Bergers, & Defares, 1986). In the current study, we tested how trajectories of BMI development in adolescence were related to differences in eating behaviour.

According to the theory of restrained eating, dieting can cause overweight through overeating. People who diet suppress their feeling of hunger cognitively and eat less. However, when cognitions of self-control are undermined restrained eaters may abandon their diet altogether and are more likely to overeat than non-dieting individuals (Herman and Polivy, 1980, Herman and Polivy, 2004, Polivy, 1996, Polivy and Herman, 1985, Ruderman, 1986). The psychosomatic theory focuses on “emotional eating” which is overeating in response to emotional arousal or (di)stress. In other words, eating as a coping mechanism in order to reduce negative emotions (Macht & Simons, 2011). This is considered an a-typical reaction since the typical response to distress is considered loss of appetite due to physiologic effects (including inhibition of gastric contractions and elevation of blood sugar) (Greeno and Wing, 1994, Kaplan and Kaplan, 1957, Schachter et al., 1968). Emotional eating may occur in people who developed a conditioned association between negative mood and food reward, for example due to childhood experiences of food being used as comfort (Bruch, 1973) or have not learned to express and regulate their emotions in childhood (Topham et al., 2011). A third theory, focussing on external eating, states that certain people are more sensible to external food cues than others, and eat in response to a variety of external stimuli (Schachter & Rodin, 1974), such as time manipulations, taste, visibility and accessibility of food, regardless of internal states of feelings of hunger or satiety. In an obesogenic environment with many (unhealthy) eating cues, external eating is hypothesised to be related to overeating. The predictive value of these three theories in explaining weight status and weight change has been widely tested in adult samples (e.g. Koenders & van Strien, 2011), but in the past two decades increasing attention has been paid to children and adolescents.

Concerning restrained eating, there is consistent empirical evidence that restraint is higher in overweight adolescents. Cross-sectional studies on small samples of UK, French, and Dutch adolescents (Lluch et al., 2000, Rutters et al., 2011, Wardle et al., 1992) reveal a positive association between restraint and overweight status or BMI. In large samples of 2474 Belgium 13–18 year olds, (Braet et al., 2008) and 10,087 Dutch 12–14 year olds, (Snoek, van Strien, Janssens, & Engels, 2007) higher restrained eating scores were found for overweight adolescents. Recently, positive associations have been found between dietary restraint and several adiposity measures (BMI, waist circumference, and skinfolds) for 17–18 year old adolescent boys and girls (Ledoux, Watson, Baranowski, Tepper, & Baranowski, 2011). In prospective studies, restrained eating also predicted increases in weight in a US sample of 13–17 year olds (Stice, 2001), and similar findings were obtained for US pre-adolescent and adolescents over 3-years-time (Field et al., 2003; see also Neumark-Sztainer et al., 2007).

Fewer studies have been conducted on emotional eating in adolescents. Most of these studies found no associations between emotional eating and weight status in this age-group (Belcher et al., 2011, Goldfield et al., 2010, Lluch et al., 2000, Nguyen-Rodriguez et al., 2008, Snoek, van Strien, et al., 2007, Wardle et al., 1992) whereas Braet et al. (2008) found elevated levels of emotional eating for overweight 13–18 year old girls – but not boys, and Snoek, van Strien, et al. (2007) found a negative association with higher emotional eating scores for adolescent boys with lower BMI-scores. There is a considerable amount of research on the relationship between emotional eating and weight status in younger children. The results of these studies show both positive (Jahnke and Warschburger, 2008, Parkinson et al., 2010, Viana et al., 2008) and negative associations (Braet et al., 2008, Ledoux et al., 2011). So far, there is no prospective research on emotional eating and changes in BMI in the course of childhood and adolescence.

The few studies that did focus on external eating found that high external eating is related to lower BMI (Braet et al., 2008, Goldfield et al., 2010, Lluch et al., 2000, Snoek, van Strien, et al., 2007, Wardle et al., 1992) or for younger children unrelated to weight status (Hill et al., 1994, Jahnke and Warschburger, 2008, Ledoux et al., 2011), which is in sharp contrast with experimental research showing that food cues are related to food intake in children (Anschutz, Engels, & Van Strien, 2009) and to food intake and being overweight in adults (Herman and Polivy, 2008, van Strien, Herman, Anschutz, 2012, Wansink et al., 2007, Wardle et al., 1992). In addition, external eating is related to enhanced selective attention (i.e. attentional bias) to food cues (Brignell, Griffiths, Bradley, & Mogg, 2009) which suggests a higher vulnerability to overeating (although others suggest this relation might be due to impulsivity, see Hou et al., 2011). The relation between external eating and weight status is thus unclear especially as survey studies on the associations between external eating and weight status in youth so far have been cross-sectional and prospective studies are lacking.

To test the relation of the eating behaviours with body weight status, it is essential to have longitudinal data in which people are followed over the course of adolescence. Although there is ample research testing age differences in weight status or body weight (e.g. Cole, Bellizzi, Flegal, & Dietz, 2000), there is a lack of studies that examine individual differences in the development of body weight in adolescence. In the area of developmental psychology, growth mixture modelling is a state-of-the-art technique to test whether clusters of individuals exist within a population who follow unique, qualitatively distinct developmental trajectories (i.e. latent trajectory groups; for a detailed description see, e.g., Jones et al., 2001, Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2012, Nagin, 1999). A few studies applied this technique on weight status of adolescents. For example, Li, Goran, Kaur, Nollen, and Ahluwalia (2007) found three trajectories of overweight trajectories: early onset overweight, late onset overweight and never overweight of which the two overweight trajectories were predicted by among others, socio-demographic and maternal lifestyle variables. Mustillo et al. (2003) found four trajectories of obesity, only chronic obesity was associated with psychiatric disorders. Finally, Ventura, Loken, and Birch (2009) found four trajectories of BMI development for adolescent girls, the highest BMI trajectory was related to a higher maternal BMI and shorter duration of being breast-fed. No study so far related eating behaviours of adolescents to developmental trajectories of BMI. This has an added value to the current literature on emotional, external and restrained eating in adolescence. Firstly because prospective studies relating emotional and external eating to BMI are lacking. Secondly, because with this technique not only a positive association can be shown, but also how eating behaviour relates to different patterns of BMI development.

Using growth mixture modelling, we computed the individual body weight trajectories of 328 Dutch adolescents, using data from five annual waves. The first aim of this study is to explore developmental trajectories of BMI in adolescence. The second aim is to examine how eating behaviours at baseline measurement (when adolescents are approximately 13–15 year old) are related to individual membership to one of the BMI trajectories. We controlled for parental BMI since associations between eating behaviour and BMI could be affected by family similarities in BMI (Davison, Markey, & Birch, 2000) and eating behaviour, especially for emotional (Snoek, Engels, Janssens, & van Strien, 2007) and restrained eating (Snoek, van Strien, Janssens, & Engels, 2009).

Section snippets

Procedure

Participants of this study were children from 328 Dutch families who participated in the 5 year longitudinal ‘Family and Health’ study on individual and family predictors of various health-related behaviours of adolescents. For more detailed information on participant selection and characteristics see Harakeh et al., 2005, van der Vorst et al., 2005, and Snoek, van Strien, Janssens, & Engels, 2008.

Families with at least two children aged between 13 and 16 years were selected from the registers of

Step 1: trajectory models of body mass index

Table 1 shows BMI scores at each wave for the total group and for boys and girls separately. It appears that boys and girls do not differ on BMI level on any of the time points, accept for T1 when, girls have somewhat higher BMI scores than boys (p < 0.05).

The BIC indicated that a five-group solution (BIC = 3082.18) had a superior fit to the data compared to a four-group model (BIC = 3134.32), a three-group model (BIC = 3230.72) or a two-group model (BIC = 3554.94).

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to determine how eating behaviours of adolescents were related to different BMI developmental trajectories, by using growth mixture modelling; a method to identify clusters of individuals within a population that follow distinct developmental trajectories. In this study the development of BMI from early to mid-adolescence was best fitted in five parallel but distinct trajectories that differed in magnitude but not in shape: all trajectories showed moderate

References (77)

  • K.N. Parkinson et al.

    Do maternal ratings of appetite in infants predict later Child Eating Behaviour Questionnaire scores and body mass index

    Appetite

    (2010)
  • J. Polivy

    Psychological consequences of food restriction

    Journal of the American Dietetics Association

    (1996)
  • K. Raspopow et al.

    Psychosocial stressor effects on cortisol and ghrelin in emotional and non-emotional eaters. Influence on anger and shame

    Hormones and Behaviour

    (2010)
  • H.M. Snoek et al.

    Parental behaviour and adolescents’ emotional eating

    Appetite

    (2007)
  • H.M. Snoek et al.

    Longitudinal relationships between fathers’, mothers’, and adolescents’ restrained eating

    Appetite

    (2009)
  • E. Stice

    Prospective relation of dieting behaviors to weight change in a community sample of adolescents

    Behavior Therapy

    (1998)
  • G.L. Topham et al.

    Parenting styles, parental response to child emotion, and family emotional responsiveness are related to child emotional eating

    Appetite

    (2011)
  • T. van Strien et al.

    Moderation of distress-induced eating by emotional eating scores

    Appetite

    (2012)
  • T. van Strien et al.

    On the relationship between emotional and external eating behavior

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1995)
  • D.J. Wallis et al.

    Stress and eating. The effects of ego-threat and cognitive demand on food intake in restrained and emotional eaters

    Appetite

    (2004)
  • D.J. Wallis et al.

    Emotions and eating. Self-reported and experimentally induced changes in food intake under stress

    Appetite

    (2009)
  • J. Wardle et al.

    Eating style and eating behaviour in adolescents

    Appetite

    (1992)
  • C. Braet et al.

    Differences in eating style between overweight and normal-weight youngsters

    Journal of Health Psychology

    (2008)
  • H. Bruch

    Hunger awareness and individuation

  • T.J. Cole et al.

    Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide. International survey

    British Medical Journal

    (2000)
  • E. Delgade-Rico et al.

    BMI predicts emotion-driven impulsivity and cognitive inflexibility in adolescents with excess weight

    Obesity

    (2012)
  • C.K. Enders et al.

    The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models

    Structural Equation Modeling

    (2001)
  • C. Evers et al.

    Assessing yourself as an emotional eater. Mission impossible?

    Health Psychology

    (2009)
  • A.E. Field et al.

    Relation between dieting and weight change among preadolescents and adolescents

    Pediatrics

    (2003)
  • S. Fischer et al.

    Clarifying the role of impulsivity in bulimia nervosa

    International Journal of Eating Disorders

    (2003)
  • A.M. Fredriks et al.

    Body index measurements in 1996–7 compared with 1980

    Archives of Disease in Childhood

    (2000)
  • G.S. Goldfield et al.

    Body dissatisfaction, dietary restraint, depression, and weight status in adolescents

    Journal of School Health

    (2010)
  • L. Goossens et al.

    Loss of control over eating in overweight youngsters. The role of anxiety, depression, and emotional eating

    European Eating Disorders Review

    (2009)
  • C.G. Greeno et al.

    Stress-induced eating

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1994)
  • Z. Harakeh et al.

    Parental rules and communication. Their association with adolescent smoking

    Addiction

    (2005)
  • P.C.L. Heaven et al.

    Neuroticism and conscientiousness as predictors of emotional, external, and restrained eating behaviors

    International Journal of Eating Disorders

    (2001)
  • C.P. Herman et al.

    The self-regulation of eating. Theoretical and practical problems

  • C.P. Herman et al.

    Restrained eating

  • Cited by (54)

    • Maternal sensitivity in early childhood and body mass index in adolescence: A population-based study on the role of self-regulation as a mediator

      2023, Appetite
      Citation Excerpt :

      Additionally, both general and food-specific parenting, for instance by non-sensitive, controlling feeding practices, may lead to restrained eating in offspring, which could ultimately frustrate the ability to self-regulate offspring's own eating (Birch & Fisher, 1998; Kral & Rauh, 2010). Adolescents are especially prone to restrained eating when they have an avid appetite or relatively high BMI (Snoek, Engels, Van Strien, & Otten, 2013; Snoek, van Strien, Janssens, & Engels, 2008). So far, however, a comprehensive model including parenting, children's self-regulation, eating behaviour, and BMI has not been studied.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Acknowledgements: The “Family and Health” Project was supported by grants from the Dutch Cancer Society (2005-3435) and the Dutch Organization of Scientific Research (400-05-051; 016-005-029).

    View full text