Elsevier

Appetite

Volume 59, Issue 3, December 2012, Pages 782-789
Appetite

Research report
Eating style, overeating and weight gain. A prospective 2-year follow-up study in a representative Dutch sample

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.009Get rights and content

Abstract

This study examined which individuals are particularly at risk for developing overweight and whether there are behavioral lifestyle factors that may attenuate this susceptibility. A prospective study with a 2-year follow-up was conducted in a sample representative of the general population of The Netherlands (n = 590). Body mass change (self-reported) was assessed in relation to overeating and change in physical activity (both self-reported), dietary restraint, emotional eating, and external eating, as assessed by the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire. There was a consistent main (suppressive) effect of increased physical activity on BMI change. Only emotional eating and external eating moderated the relation between overeating and body mass change. However, the interaction effect of external eating became borderline significant with Yes or No meaningful weight gain (weight gain >3%) as dependent variable. It was concluded that whilst increasing physical activity may attenuate weight gain, particularly high emotional eaters seem at risk for developing overweight, because overconsumption seems to be more strongly related to weight gain in people with high degrees of emotional eating.

Highlights

► A prospective study (2-year follow-up) in a representative Dutch sample. ► Body mass change, overeating, eating styles and physical activity. ► Increasing physical activity attenuated weight gain. ► Emotional eating moderated the association of overeating with body mass change. ► Particularly high emotional eaters seem at risk for developing overweight.

Introduction

The current environment has been described as obesogenic (Swinburn, Egger, & Raza, 1999), meaning that abundant availability of food, coupled with declines in physical activity in interaction with genetic susceptibility, encourage positive energy balance, weight gain, and ultimately overweight. Despite the potency of this obesogenic environment, not all people become overweight; some remain lean. Susceptibility to increased body weight may be understood at many levels, ranging from genetic, physiological or metabolic, to behavioral and psychological (Blundell et al., 2005). The present study focuses on behavioral and psychological factors, and examines which individuals are particularly at risk for developing overweight and whether there are behavioral lifestyle factors that may exacerbate or attenuate this susceptibility.

Increasing physical activity and reducing food intake (dieting) are considered cornerstones in the prevention and treatment of obesity (Holmes et al., 2010, Keith et al., 2006). Yet dietary restraint1 has been found to be associated with excessive food intake and weight gain (Chaput et al., 2009, Polivy and Herman, 1985, Stice et al., 1999). Also reviews of calorie-restricted diets have not provided grounds for optimism regarding the effectiveness of such diets in the long term (Aphramor, 2010, Mann et al., 2007). In the Mann et al. (2007) meta-analysis, between one-third and two-thirds of the dieters had at follow-up regained more weight than they lost on their diets. A problem with dietary restraint is that the body cannot distinguish true food shortage from self-imposed food restriction and acts as if it is in the starvation mode: feelings of hunger increase and metabolic rate slows down (anabolism and adaptive thermogenesis) (Goldsmith et al., 2010, Major et al., 2007). Moreover, dietary restraint (a form of inhibition) is often associated with overeating tendencies (disinhibition), as in emotional eating or external eating. Emotional eating refers to the tendency to overeat in response to negative emotions as result from poor interoceptive awareness, a notion derived from Bruch’s (1964), psychosomatic theory of obesity. External eating refers to the overeating tendency resulting from susceptibility to tempting food cues, a notion derived from Schachter’s (1968) externality theory of obesity (Herman & Polivy, 2008). Emotional and/or external eating can therefore complicate the association between dietary restraint, food intake, and change in body weight. What ought to produce weight loss may end up producing weight gain (Herman, van Strien, & Polivy, 2008). The precise direction of the relationship between dietary restraint and these overeating tendencies is as yet unclear and may even differ for various subgroups (Spoor et al., 2006, Stice, 1998, Stice et al., 2002, van Strien et al., 2005).

The risk of anabolism and weight (re)gain after a diet may be counteracted by increasing physical activity. What is more, physical activity may have benefits that go beyond increased caloric expenditure and increasing metabolic rate, because it has been found to be associated with lower depressive symptomatology, decreased feelings of tension, and greater emotional well-being (Amenesi and Whitaker, 2008, Dunn et al., 2005). Physical activity was found to be negatively associated with emotional and external eating (van Strien & Koenders, 2010), and physical activity self-efficacy (i.e., confidence in one’s ability to be regularly physically active) was also negatively associated with emotional eating (Konttinen, Silventoinen, Sarlio-Lähteenkorva, Männistö, & Haukkala, 2010).

For the present study we were interested in extending prospectively earlier cross-sectional results regarding the moderator effects of restrained, emotional, and external eating on the relation between overconsumption and overweight. In this earlier study in a representative Dutch sample (van Strien, Herman, & Verheijden, 2009), both dietary restraint and emotional overeating significantly moderated the relationship between overconsumption and level of overweight. Overconsumption was more strongly related to overweight in people with lower levels of dietary restraint and in people with higher levels of emotional eating. External eating, however, did not moderate the relationship between overconsumption and level of overweight, and there also was no positive main effect of external eating on level of overweight. It was concluded that one’s body weight is possibly determined more by one’s tendency toward emotional eating than by one’s sensitivity to environmental food cues. A further conclusion was that dietary restraint may prevent people who overeat from developing overweight.

The finding that overweight people did not differ from normal-weight people in their degree of external eating is somewhat surprising in view of the recent interest in the possible role of environmental (external) food cues in the development of overweight (the obesogenic environment!) (Herman & Polivy, 2008). The absence of a difference between overweight and normal weight people in external eating is, however, consistent with similar results in various other studies (Lluch et al., 2000, Pothos et al., 2009, Snoek et al., 2007b, van Strien et al., 2007, Wardle, 1987, Wardle et al., 1992). Also in a prospective 4-year follow-up study on 1576 adult Korean twins and their families, there was no support for external eating as risk factor for development of overweight; external eating was not associated with either overweight or weight gain (Sung, Lee, & Song, 2009). The same result was obtained in a prospective 2-year follow-up study on 1562 employees in a Dutch banking environment (Koenders & van Strien, 2011). So the question now arises: Is the absence of support for external eating as a risk factor for overweight and weight gain robust and can this finding be replicated in a prospective study in a different representative Dutch sample?

The finding by van Strien et al. (2009) that dietary restraint attenuated the relation between overconsumption and overweight is also in need of replication in a prospective study, because it conflicts with outcomes of earlier studies in which dietary restraint was shown to be an important risk-factor for overeating and weight gain (Mann et al., 2007, Polivy and Herman, 1985). Also of interest would be the assessment of the possible attenuating role of physical activity in the development of overweight. Results of a recent meta-analysis of diet interventions suggested that the risk of regaining body weight was lower in a diet-plus-exercise intervention than in a diet-alone intervention (Wu, Gao, Chen, & van Dam, 2009). Further, in the prospective 2-year follow-up study in Dutch banking employees, a high level of athletics was found to be predictive of weight loss. A further finding in that study was that the positive association of emotional eating with weight gain was weaker in employees with high engagement in sports than in those with low engagement in sports (Koenders & van Strien, 2011). Accordingly, it would be of interest to ascertain whether physical activity has an attenuating effect on the association between over-consumption and weight gain.

In the present prospective study on a representative Dutch sample, overconsumption, dietary restraint, emotional eating, external eating, and physical activity were assessed in spring 2009 (T1) and tested against BMI in spring 2011 (T2), controlling for spring 2009 (T1) BMI. The following hypotheses were formulated. Dietary restraint and physical activity were both expected to attenuate the positive relationship between overeating and body mass change after 2 years, whereas emotional eating was expected to strengthen this relationship. No moderator effect was expected for external eating. We further expected these findings to remain robust in the models that included the other eating styles as possible confounders.

Section snippets

Participants

Data were collected in a cohort of Dutch adults (representative for age, sex, SES, ethnic origin, and region in The Netherlands) as part of a larger longitudinal study on knowledge and use of the Dutch mass media campaign: Monitoring Healthy body weight. Participants were recruited through a panel service agency. At baseline, a sample of 1200 participants was recruited.

At the first measurement (T1) of the present study, height and weight measures were available for 744 participants (64% of the

Mean differences

Means and standard deviations of all variables may be found in Table 1 (low versus high overconsumers at base line) and Table 2 (normal-weight and overweight participants at baseline). There were no differences between high and low overconsumers in regard to age and sex. On all other variables the means of high overconsumers were significantly higher on all variables than those of low overconsumers (see Table 1), except for physical activity, where percentage of norm-active participants was

Discussion

The present study replicated an earlier cross-sectional study (van Strien et al., 2009) in a representative Dutch sample this time using a prospective 2-year follow-up design. In addition to overconsumption, emotional eating, external eating, and dietary restraint, physical activity was also assessed.

As in the earlier study, high overconsumers differed from low overconsumers in that they were more often overweight, had higher degrees of dietary restraint, emotional eating and external eating,

References (57)

  • G.L. Topham et al.

    Parenting styles, parental response to child emotion, and family emotional responsiveness are related to child emotional eating

    Appetite

    (2011)
  • T. van Strien et al.

    Moderation of distress-induced eating by emotional eating scores

    Appetite

    (2012)
  • T. van Strien et al.

    Eating style, overeating and overweight in a representative Dutch sample. Does external eating play a role?

    Appetite

    (2009)
  • T. van Strien et al.

    Parental control and the Dopamine D2 Receptor Gene (DRD2) interaction on emotional eating in adolescence

    Appetite

    (2010)
  • T. van Strien et al.

    Intake of energy is best predicted by overeating tendency and consumption of fat is best predicted by dietary restraint. A 4-year follow up of patients with newly diagnosed type-2 diabetes

    Appetite

    (2008)
  • T. van Strien et al.

    Emotional eating in adolescents. A gene (SLC6A4/5-HTT)-depressive feelings interaction analysis

    Journal of Psychiatric Research

    (2010)
  • J. Wardle

    Eating style. A validation study of the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire in normal weight subjects and women with eating disorders

    Journal of Psychosomatic Research

    (1987)
  • J. Wardle et al.

    Eating style and eating behaviour in adolescents

    Appetite

    (1992)
  • J. Amenesi et al.

    Relations of mood and exercise with weight loss in formerly sedentary obese women

    American Journal of Health Behavior

    (2008)
  • L. Aphramor

    Validity of claims made in weight management research. A narrative review of dietetic articles

    Nutrition Journal

    (2010)
  • CBS. Central Bureau voor de Statistiek (2009). CBS statline....
  • J.P. Chaput et al.

    Risk factors for adult overweight and obesity in the Quebec Family Study. Have we been barking up the wrong three?

    Obesity

    (2009)
  • V. Drapeau et al.

    Do 6-y changes in eating behaviors predict changes in body weight? Results from the Québec Family Study

    International Journal of Obesity

    (2003)
  • P.W. Gold et al.

    Organization of the stress system and its dysregulation in melancholic and atypical depression. High vs. low CRH/NE states

    Molecular Psychiatry

    (2002)
  • R. Goldsmith et al.

    Effects of experimental weight perturbation on skeletal work efficiency, fuel utilization, and biochemistry in human subjects

    American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology

    (2010)
  • W.L. Haskell et al.

    Updated recommendation for adults. From the American college of sports, medicine and the American heart association

    Circulation

    (2007)
  • C.P. Herman et al.

    Undereating or eliminating overeating?

    American Psychologist

    (2008)
  • G.N. Holmbeck

    Post-hoc probing of significant moderational and mediational effects in studies of pediatric populations

    Journal of Pediatric Psychology

    (2002)
  • Cited by (130)

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Acknowledgements: We thank The Netherlands Nutrition Centre for allowing secondary data analysis and the Dutch Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sports for funding Marieke W. Verheijden.

    View full text