Elsevier

Appetite

Volume 58, Issue 2, April 2012, Pages 554-558
Appetite

The contrasting physiological and subjective effects of chewing gum on social stress

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.11.013Get rights and content

Abstract

Uncertainty exists with respect to the extent to which chewing gum may attenuate stress-induced rises in cortisol secretion (Johnson et al., 2011, Scholey et al., 2009, Smith, 2010). The present study used the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST: Kirschbaum, Pirke, & Hellhammer, 1993), a task known to elevate cortisol secretion (Kudielka, Schommer, Hellhammer, & Kirschbaum, 2004), in order to examine the moderating physiological and subjective effects of chewing gum on social stress. Forty participants completed the TSST either with or without chewing gum. As expected, completion of the TSST elevated both cortisol and subjective stress levels, whilst impairing mood. Although gum moderated the perception of stress, cortisol concentrations were higher following the chewing of gum. The findings are consistent with Smith (2010) who argued that elevations in cortisol following the chewing of gum reflect heightened arousal. The findings suggest that chewing gum only benefits subjective measures of stress. The mechanism remains unclear; however, this may reflect increased cerebral blood flow, cognitive distraction, and/or effects secondary to task facilitation.

Introduction

In a recent study, Sketchley-Kaye, Jenks, Miles, and Johnson (2011) examined the moderating effect of chewing gum on both anxiety and mood following exposure to the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST: Kirschbaum et al., 1993). The task, involving an oral presentation and mental arithmetic test, induced a decrease in both self-rated calmness and contentedness and increases in state-anxiety. Chewing gum acted to moderate this increase in state-anxiety whilst elevating self-rated alertness. This pattern of findings complements an earlier study (Scholey et al., 2009) in which chewing gum moderated the rise in both self-rated stress (see also Smith, 2009a, Smith, 2010) and state anxiety, and significantly decreased cortisol concentrations following a cognitive-load stressor.

The reduction in stress levels, both physiologically and subjectively, following the chewing of gum can be interpreted via the mastication-induced changes in cerebral blood flow (e.g. Fang, Li, Lu, Gong, & Yew, 2005). Indeed, heightened delivery of both oxygen and glucose to fronto–temporal regions (Onozuka et al., 2002) can act to increase metabolic rate, a post-stress process (in areas BA9 and BA10) associated with a reduction in salivary cortisol concentrations (Kern et al., 2008).

Notwithstanding the above proposed mechanism, in a similar design, Johnson et al. (2011) failed to replicate Scholey et al. (2009) in that chewing gum did not attenuate the rise in cortisol. Furthermore, Smith (2010) reported that chewing gum elevated cortisol concentrations under conditions of acute (noise-induced) stress: a finding taken to reflect the heightened alertness/arousal following the chewing of gum (see also Johnson et al., 2011, Onyper et al., 2011, Scholey et al., 2009, Smith, 2009b, for alertness effects). These studies suggest a degree of unreliability in respect to the effects of gum on cortisol. One possibility for such unreliability is the diurnal variation in cortisol excretion. For instance, Johnson et al. (2011) tested participants in the morning when cortisol levels are typically high (Hucklebridge, Hussain, Evans, & Clow, 2005) and such elevation may have masked the effects of chewing and/or the stressful task. Indeed, in Scholey et al. (2009), cortisol levels fell following the stressor when participants were tested in the morning (corroborating the Johnson et al. (2011) data). However, since Scholey et al. (2009) did not include time of day in their analysis of gum effects, it is unclear the extent to which the reductive effects of chewing gum on cortisol were confined to the afternoon. Similarly, in Smith (2010), despite testing taking place at intervals across the day, time of day was not included as a variable in his analysis. It is, therefore, unclear whether time of day differentially influenced the cortisol excretion. Additionally, Smith (2010) does not report whether there was a main effect of the noise stressor on cortisol excretion. That is, we do not know if the noise was ‘physiologically’ stressful. If not, then the scenario is quite different to that of Scholey et al. (2009), in which the rise in cognitive-load induced cortisol excretion was mediated via the chewing of gum.

The present study examines the extent to which increases in cortisol excretion are moderated by chewing gum with a task known to produce established and reliable effects on cortisol excretion (TSST: Kirschbaum et al., 1993; see also Kudielka et al., 2004). The presentation and mental arithmetic components of the TSST, in particular, have been shown to increase both cortisol excretion (Kudielka et al., 2004) and state-anxiety (Sketchley-Kaye et al., 2011), and decrease subjective mood state (Kudielka et al., 2004, Sketchley-Kaye et al., 2011). As aforementioned, Sketchley-Kaye et al. (2011) found that chewing gum attenuated the rise in self-rated anxiety following the TSST. In the present study we examine the extent to which this mediating effect of gum following the TSST can be extended to cortisol and self-rated stress.

The TSST comprises a social evaluative stress task where participants deliver a presentation and perform a mental arithmetic task in front of a panel. There are four stages to the task: baseline measures, preparation for the presentation, presentation/mental arithmetic stressor, and post-task recovery. To the extent that chewing gum affects cortisol excretion under conditions of acute physiological stress (Scholey et al., 2009), we predict an interaction between the experimental stage and chewing gum condition. That is, we expect differences between the gum and no gum groups at specific points in the stressor protocol. One might expect increases in stress immediately prior to the TSST (anticipatory stress) and immediately following the TSST. If chewing gum attenuates the rise in cortisol production under conditions of acute stress (Scholey et al., 2009), lower cortisol levels should be reported in the gum group at these stages. Furthermore, since a temporal delay exists in respect to stress exposure and salivary cortisol peak (Kudielka et al., 2004), one might predict differences between the gum and no gum groups following the recovery phase due to a delay in cortisol returning to normal levels. Additionally, if chewing gum moderates the stress induced changes in self-rated stress and mood (e.g. Scholey et al., 2009, Sketchley-Kaye et al., 2011), we predict an interaction between chewing gum and experimental stage such that increases in stress and decreases in mood are both attenuated in the chewing gum condition.

Section snippets

Participants

Forty (20 males, 20 females, mean age = 20 years and 3 months) non-smoking Coventry University Psychology undergraduates participated in exchange for course credit. All participants reported that they were free from both concurrent medication (including the contraceptive pill) and illicit drug use. Participants were instructed to refrain from caffeine, alcohol, and chewing gum on the day of testing and asked to not consume food for up to 1 h prior to testing. Participants were assigned at random to

Group differences

To confirm a null difference in baseline trait anxiety scores between the gum (mean = 38.55; SEM = 2.12) and no-gum (mean = 41.50; SEM = 1.70) groups their scores were compared via an independent samples 2-tailed t-test (t(38) = 1.08, p = 0.29).

Statistics

A 2-way (2 × 4) mixed ANOVA was employed on all dependent variables with the between-participants factor chewing gum group (gum and no gum) and the within-participants factor experimental stage (baseline, pre-TSST, post-TSST, and post 10-min recovery).

Effects of stress task

The effects of

Discussion

We employed an established cortisol-elevating task (TSST: Kirschbaum et al., 1993; see also Kudielka et al., 2004) in order to examine the extent to which chewing gum can act to attenuate the rise in stress-induced cortisol concentrations. In line with predictions, completion of the TSST, in comparison to the baseline score, produced significant increases in cortisol excretion and this increase was similar to self-rated increases in both stress and anxiety, and decreases in both contentedness

References (20)

There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (16)

  • Masticatory performance alters stress relief effect of gum chewing

    2015, Journal of Prosthodontic Research
    Citation Excerpt :

    The validity of salivary CgA levels as an indicator of stress has been confirmed by experimental stress tests, including cognitive tests, noise exposure, and venipuncture [20–22]. Many previous studies [6–13] on the stress-relieving effects of gum chewing measured salivary cortisol as the stress index. Because the response to stress mediated by the HPA is affected by the menstrual cycle [23], some of these reports included only male participants [7–9,12].

  • New Techniques for Augmenting Saliva Collection: Bacon Rules and Lozenge Drools

    2015, Clinical Therapeutics
    Citation Excerpt :

    For instance, Scholey et al22 found that chewing gum during laboratory stress was associated with reduced perceived and lower salivary cortisol. Gray et al23 similarly found that chewing gum during a stressful task reduced subjective measures of stress but heightened cortisol levels. However, others have failed to find this attenuation of perceived stress.24

  • Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress Test

    2014, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    Citation Excerpt :

    The TSST increases reported stress (Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 2002b; Sugaya et al., 2012), and anxiety (Jezova et al., 2004; Rimmele et al., 2007, 2009; Rohrmann et al., 1999; von Dawans et al., 2011), and reduces reported calmness (Childs et al., 2006; Kirschbaum et al., 1999). Administration of the TSST also leads to higher wakefulness (Kirschbaum et al., 1999), although the TSST did not increase reported alertness elsewhere (Gray et al., 2012; Sketchley-Kaye et al., 2011). Broader measures of mood have also been used; the TSST worsened negative mood as assessed by the positive and negative affect scale (PANAS) (Firk and Markus, 2009; Yim et al., 2010) as well as on the multidimensional mood questionnaire (Rimmele et al., 2007, 2009) and on a visual analogue scale (Childs et al., 2006).

View all citing articles on Scopus

Acknowledgements: The authors would like to thank Dr. Lara Tucha for her comments on an earlier draft and the later comments from both Prof. Debra Zellner and an anonymous reviewer.

View full text