Elsevier

Addictive Behaviors

Volume 63, December 2016, Pages 89-92
Addictive Behaviors

Short Communication
Effects of parental monitoring on alcohol use in the US and Sweden: A brief report

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.07.014Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Parental monitoring is related to lower alcohol use in adolescents.

  • Country may moderate parental monitoring, so we compared parental monitoring's relation to alcohol use in the US/Sweden.

  • Main effects of country & PM, interaction between country & PM predicting drinks/week, & PM predicting problem alcohol use.

Abstract

Objective

Adolescent alcohol use predicts a myriad of negative mental and physical health outcomes including fatality (Midanik, 2004). Research in parental influence on alcohol consumption finds parental monitoring (PM), or knowing where/whom your child is with, is associated with lower levels of alcohol use in adolescents (e.g., Arria et al., 2008). As PM interventions have had only limited success (Koutakis, Stattin, & Kerr, 2008), investigating moderating factors of PM is of importance. Country may serve as one such moderator (Calafat, Garcia, Juan, Becoña, & Fernández-Hermida, 2014). Thus, the purpose of the present report is to assess the relationship between PM and alcohol use in the US and Sweden.

Method

High school seniors from the US (n = 1181, 42.3% Male) and Sweden (n = 2171, 44.1% Male) completed assessments of total drinks consumed in a typical week, problematic alcohol use, and perceived PM.

Results

Generalized linear mixed modeling (GLM, Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2013; Hilbe, 2011) was used to examine whether country moderated the relationship between PM and alcohol use. Results revealed main effects of country and PM and a significant interaction between country and PM in predicting total drinks per week and PM in predicting problematic alcohol use (p < 0.001).

Conclusions

While PM is related to lower quantity of alcohol consumed and problematic alcohol use, greater PM appears to be more strongly related to fewer drinks per week and less problematic alcohol use in the US, as compared to Sweden.

Introduction

Early alcohol use is a robust predictor of physical/mental health outcomes in adolescents, including drug/alcohol dependence, depression, aggression, and victimization (e.g., McGue and Iacono, 2008, Midanik, 2004). Underage drinking is a leading causes of death among youth (National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, 2007), and directly responsible for higher rates of physical injury and high-risk sexual activity. Despite clear evidence that one aspect of parenting, parental monitoring (PM), is associated with lower levels of adolescent drinking (Arria et al., 2008, Beck et al., 2004), prevention efforts that have targeted PM have had only limited success (e.g. Koutakis, Stattin, & Kerr, 2008). This highlights the importance of identifying dispositional factors that may strengthen the effect of parental monitoring on adolescent alcohol consumption. Parental styles across countries have been proposed as one such moderator (Calafat et al., 2014). Given that country of origin is not a malleable factor, it is important to understand if the efficacy of PM is affected by country. PM and alcohol has been studied in several countries (e.g., Strunin et al., 2013), but rarely across countries. Thus, the current brief report provides a preliminary examination into the extent that country is associated with adolescent sensitivity to parental monitoring.

Substantial evidence suggests adolescent drinking practices differ across countries (Norström, 2002, Hibell et al., 2004, Rehm et al., 2003, Ahlstrom and Osterberg, 2004). Compared with American youth, European adolescents initiate drinking earlier and are half as likely to abstain from alcohol, than American adolescents (Jernigan, 2001, Johnston et al., 2005, Hvitfeldt et al., 2004). According to the European School Survey Project on Alcohol and Other Drug Use (ESPAD) report, youth in Sweden reported drinking more than average amounts of alcohol when compared to other European countries (7.0 versus 5.1 cL). Given the central role culture plays in parenting and adolescent drinking, country may be critical in determining the association between PM and adolescent problem drinking. Research on parenting in the US and Sweden suggests differing parental styles. According to Sorbring and Gurdal (2011), in Sweden more equal responsibilities and opportunities are spread between children and parents. Thus, Swedish parents “negotiate” with their children. In contrast, in the US the hierarchy between individuals is highlighted. Thus, individuals are more competitive and power dynamic.

Dishon and McMahon (1998) proposed a triad of interrelated components of parenting that impact youth problem behavior: motivation, behavior management, and parental monitoring (PM), defined as “parenting behaviors involving attention to and tracking of the child's whereabouts, activities, and adaptations.” Indeed, evidence suggests that PM has been protective for a range of adolescent risk behaviors (Lahey et al., 2008, Fletcher et al., 2004, Bohnert et al., 2012). Critically, two-decades of research have revealed that high levels of PM are consistently associated with lower levels of adolescent drinking and related consequences in the US (Arria et al., 2008, Beck et al., 2004, Barnes et al., 1994, Fors et al., 1999, Griffin et al., 2000, Branstetter and Furman, 2013), over and above peer influence (Steinberg, Fletcher, & Darling, 1994). However, it has been noted that many of the studies that purport to examine parental monitoring have been limited by only examining one component of this process, parental knowledge. This dimension of parental monitoring has come under criticism for being a more passive measure of child disclosure rather than active parental tracking and monitoring of youth behaviors (e.g. Kerr et al., 2010, Stattin and Kerr, 2000), highlighting the need to assess not only parental knowledge but also active limit-setting that accompanies it to fully measure active parental monitoring. Indeed, studies that have examined parental knowledge alone have found inconsistent results. While some research suggests that parental knowledge alone can be effective at buffering youth from drinking problems (Kerr et al., 2010), other research indicates that it may be an insufficient protective factor (e.g. for females, Barnes et al., 2005, Borawski et al., 2003, Griffin et al., 2000). Thus, the present study incorporates rule setting with parental knowledge items in order to more closely approximate PM. These inconsistent results across studies suggest that PM, and factors determining adolescent sensitivity to PM, remain inadequately understood.

The present study fills this gap in the literature by examining whether country moderates the relation between PM, indicated by both parental knowledge and rule-setting, and adolescent problem drinking in two large community-based samples of high school seniors living in the US and in Sweden. Based on prior research in the US (Aria et al., 2008) and Sweden (Bergh, Hagquist, & Starrin, 2010), we hypothesized that higher levels of PM would be associated with lower levels of adolescent drinking in both cultures. However, we hypothesized that high levels of PM would have a stronger relation with low levels of drinking among US adolescents due to the relative authority of US parents and the lower overall adolescent drinking observed in the US compared with Sweden.

Section snippets

Materials and method

In brief, assenting 17 year-old and consenting 18 year-old high school seniors (N = 3059) from 22 high-schools across Washington State in the US and from 19 high schools in the Region of Skåne, Sweden were recruited to a 4-year study investigating a brief web-based intervention for alcohol use during adolescence and early adulthood (complete methods, Grazoli et al., 2015). Analyses were performed on baseline data collected from Washington (n = 1181, M age = 17.59, 70.5% Caucasian, 42.3% Male) and

Results

GLM results between country and PM and total drinks per week revealed a main effect of country (β = 1.08, p < 0.001) and PM (β =  0.08, p < 0.001) and a significant interaction of country and PM onto total drinks per week (β = 0.06, p < 0.001). Swedish participants reported the most total drinks per week and higher PM was related to fewer total drinks per week overall. However, higher PM was more strongly related to fewer drinks per week in the US than in Sweden. While associations were small, simple slope

Discussion

We found PM was more strongly associated with lower alcohol use and alcohol problems in the US than in Sweden. While are not large differences, parental monitoring might not have the same effects across different cultural environments. It might be that governmental policies trump parental influence on alcohol use. For instance, Sweden's alcohol policy in drinking age and strict control of access may be so effective it diminishes PM's influence on alcohol. Overall, countries with more

References (35)

  • K.M. Bohnert et al.

    Parental monitoring at age 11 and subsequent onset of cannabis use up to age 17: Results from a prospective study

    Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs

    (2012)
  • S.A. Branstetter et al.

    Buffering effect of parental monitoring knowledge and parent-adolescent relationships on consequences of adolescent substance use

    Journal of Child and Family Studies

    (2013)
  • J. Cohen et al.

    Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences

    (2013)
  • T.J. Dishon et al.

    Parental monitoring and the prevention of problem behavior: A conceptual and empirical reformulation

    Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review

    (1998)
  • A.C. Fletcher et al.

    Parental influences on adolescent problem behavior: Revisiting Stattin and Kerr

    Child Development

    (2004)
  • S.W. Fors et al.

    Key factors that protect against health risks in youth: Further evidence

    American Journal of Health Behavior

    (1999)
  • V.S. Grazioli et al.

    Use of protective behavioral strategies among US and Swedish high school drinkers: Do they moderate the relationship between drinking intentions and alcohol-related consequences?

    Psychology of Addictive Behaviors

    (2015)
  • Cited by (23)

    • Characteristics of drinking episodes associated with simultaneous alcohol and cannabis use among underage drinkers in the United States

      2023, Addictive Behaviors
      Citation Excerpt :

      We observed significantly higher odds of SAC use among those who drank in a car, in more than one location, or in someone else’s home when compared to those drinking in their own home. Although parental supervision was not assessed, these findings may add to previous research that suggests parental supervision may be a protective factor of engaging in risky behaviors (Carroll, Heleniak, & Witkiewitz, 2016; Flanagan, Auty, & Farrington, 2019; Haas, Zamboanga, Bersamin, & Hyke, 2018). However, given the age range of the sample, it is also possible that participants who did not live with a parent still had lower odds of engaging in SAC use in their own home.

    • Parent SMART (Substance Misuse in Adolescents in Residential Treatment): Pilot randomized trial of a technology-assisted parenting intervention

      2021, Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment
      Citation Excerpt :

      Given the well-documented association between adolescent drinking and school performance, it is possible that the reduction in adolescent drinking directly contributed to the reduction in school-related problems (Balsa, Giuliano, & French, 2011). It is also feasible that a third variable such as parental monitoring led to reductions in both drinking and school-related problems (Affuso, Bacchini, & Miranda, 2017; Carroll et al., 2016). Future work is needed to examine the mechanisms underlying the Parent SMART intervention.

    • Examining parental monitoring as a moderator of the relationship between depressed mood and alcohol use and problems

      2018, Addictive Behaviors
      Citation Excerpt :

      Research has supported that parental monitoring remains important for college students. Students with higher parental monitoring of alcohol use report both decreased drinking (Arria et al., 2008; Beck, Boyle, & Boekeloo, 2004; Carroll et al., 2016) as well as decreases in other risky behaviors (Barnes, Farrell, & Banerjee, 1994; Bohnert, Anthony, & Breslau, 2012; Branstetter & Furman, 2013; Fletcher et al., 2004; Fors, Crepaz, & Hayes, 1999; Griffin, Botvin, Scheier, Diaz, & Miller, 2000). These findings suggest that higher parental monitoring may be protective against increases in alcohol use over time among students with depressed mood.

    • Characteristics of drinking events associated with heavy episodic drinking among adolescents in the United States

      2017, Drug and Alcohol Dependence
      Citation Excerpt :

      Furthermore, drinking in a series of uncontrolled locations may also intensify some of the problems that result from drinking, including the risk of intoxicated driving or riding with an intoxicated driver. Such events could potentially be prevented or detected by improved parental monitoring and increased alcohol law enforcement (Carroll et al., 2016; Paschall et al., 2012; Schelleman-Offermans et al., 2012). Not surprisingly, adolescents who drank with more people were more likely to engage in HED (Bersamin et al., 2016; Clapp et al., 2006a, 2006b).

    • Parental monitoring protects against the effects of parent and adolescent depressed mood on adolescent drinking

      2017, Addictive Behaviors
      Citation Excerpt :

      Parental monitoring, defined as parental knowledge of their adolescent's whereabouts, activities, and peers (Dishion & McMahon, 1998), has been associated with reduced adolescent drinking in both cross-sectional (Carroll et al., 2016; Patrick & Schulenberg, 2013) and longitudinal studies (Nash, McQueen, & Bray, 2005; Ryan, Jorm, & Lubman, 2010; Tobler & Komro, 2010).

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This project was supported by funding from NIAAA R01AA018276 awarded to Drs. Larimer & Berglund.

    View full text