Elsevier

Addictive Behaviors

Volume 38, Issue 4, April 2013, Pages 1881-1889
Addictive Behaviors

Cue-induced cigarette craving and mixed emotions: A role for positive affect in the craving process

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.12.006Get rights and content

Abstract

Craving is an important component of nicotine addiction, and extant research has demonstrated a clear link between cue-induced craving and negative affect, with mixed results in the positive affect domain. The current study was designed to test the idea that cue-reactive craving might be associated with a mixed emotional process, or the simultaneous experience of positive and negative affect. Participants were 86 non-deprived regular smokers and tobacco chippers who provided simultaneous ratings of positive and negative affect during cue exposure to pleasant, unpleasant, neutral and cigarette cues. Results indicated that self-reported craving was elevated in response to cigarette cues compared to other valenced cue types and craving was higher to pleasant cues than either neutral or unpleasant cues. Mixed emotional responses were higher to cigarette cues than other cue types. In addition, mixed emotional responses to cigarette cues predicted craving even after controlling for smoker type, difficulties regulating negative emotion, baseline craving level and mixed emotional responses to neutral cues. As the first study to investigate mixed emotions and cigarette craving, our results highlight the importance of examining the relationship between cue-reactive craving and emotional response using models of emotion that allow for measurement of nuanced emotional experience. In addition, our findings suggest that positive affect processes may indeed play a role in craving among non-deprived smokers.

Highlights

► Simultaneous positive and negative affect reported in response to cues ► Cigarette cues associated with higher mixed emotion than other cue types ► Pleasant cues elicit higher craving than neutral or unpleasant cues ► Mixed emotions to cigarette cues predict self-reported craving ► Results suggest a role for positive affect in the cue-reactivity craving process

Introduction

Craving for cigarettes, whether induced by withdrawal or in response to cues, has been clearly linked to affective processes (Tiffany, 2009). In the realm of cue-reactivity, a robust relationship has been demonstrated between negative affect and craving (Brandon et al., 1996, Carter and Tiffany, 2001, Drobes and Tiffany, 1997, Maude-Griffin and Tiffany, 1996, Taylor et al., 2000). Despite these findings, the precise nature of the relationship between craving and affect remains uncertain. Further examination of the affective processes associated with cue response may prove important, particularly if affective responses contribute to self-reported craving and/or behavioral outcomes.

The relationship between cue-reactive craving and negative affect is clearly established in the literature. A host of evidence suggests that negative affect manipulations, even without the addition of smoking cues, prompt increased craving levels (Maude-Griffin and Tiffany, 1996, Taylor et al., 2000). Negative affect predicts craving when measured prospectively (Brandon et al., 1996, Drobes and Tiffany, 1997), and negative states show robust correlations with craving (Carter and Tiffany, 2001, Lee et al., 2007, Singleton et al., 2003). Moreover, presentation of smoking cues tends to increase negative affect more than presentation of neutral cues (Conklin et al., 2010, Doran et al., 2008).

At first blush, it appears that positive affect typically either has no correlation to smoking urge (e.g. Drobes and Tiffany, 1997, Kassel et al., 2007), or is associated with decreased urge (Bailey et al., 2010, Maude-Griffin and Tiffany, 1996, Taylor et al., 2000, Tiffany, 2009).

Theoretically, however, positive affect has been linked to urge. A recent model of impulsivity suggested that certain individuals are likely to act rashly (e.g. succumb to urges) when in a positive mood (Cyders and Smith, 2008, Cyders et al., 2007). In addition, Baker and colleagues (Baker, Morse, & Sherman, 1987) proposed a craving model with two separate urge pathways such that a positive-affect urge network may be activated by appetitive stimuli, particularly for non-deprived smokers. As an example, in a study comparing deprived and continuing smokers on craving and affect ratings over a 24-hour period, Zinser and colleagues (Zinser, Baker, Sherman, & Cannon, 1992) found a significant positive relationship between positive affect and craving for continuing smokers, whereas deprived smokers expressed concordant negative affect and craving (however, see Brandon et al., 1996 for contradictory findings).

Several of the studies that found a negative relationship between positive affect and craving have investigated the effect of positive mood, typically induced in a laboratory setting, on subsequent cue-reactive craving (Maude-Griffin and Tiffany, 1996, Taylor et al., 2000). Whether mood state influences reaction to cues is a different, albeit related, question than whether self-reported craving increases in response to valenced cues or if changes in affect follow from presentation of cigarette cues. Self-reported craving in response to valenced cues has received limited empirical attention in the realm of cue-reactivity, as the typical cue-reactivity study compares craving responses to cigarette cues with craving responses to neutral cues (Carter & Tiffany, 1999). However, recent work comparing craving responses to a full set of valenced cues (e.g., including both pleasant and unpleasant cue stimuli) found heightened craving in response to positive cues when compared to negative and neutral cues (Muñoz et al., 2010, Robinson et al., 2011). Moreover, in the realm of psychophysiology, startle eyeblink responses (where heighted responses are indicative of negative affect) are dampened to both cigarette and pleasant cues (Cui et al., 2012, Dempsey et al., 2007). Overall, these studies suggest that smoking cues are appetitive.

Other recent work has examined changes in affect following the presentation of cigarette cues, with mixed results. One study found that generalized positive affect decreased and negative affect increased in response to cigarette cue exposure (Doran et al., 2008). Another study found that smoking cues are associated with higher excitement (a specific positive feeling), when compared to non-smoking cues (Conklin et al., 2010). A third recent study examining self-reported mood in response to valenced and cigarette cues found that positive mood was higher to cigarette and positive cues compared to neutral and unpleasant cues (Robinson et al., 2011). In sum, although there is burgeoning evidence that smoking cues are appetitive, and theoretical frameworks proposing a link between positive affect and craving, the remaining empirical research on the relation between positive affect and craving is mixed. Considering that a synonym often used for craving is “desire,” which has a positive connotation, the mixed evidence is an apparent conundrum. Research focused on positive affect and craving may benefit from considering alternative approaches and methodologies to determine if, when, and for whom positive affect relates to craving.

An additional piece of the puzzle is that positive and negative affect are rarely compared to one another. For example, in a study comparing affective responses to cues, positive affect was lower in response to an imagery exposure with urge content compared to an imagery script with neutral content (Drobes & Tiffany, 1997). However, ratings of positive affect across the entire study were considerably higher than ratings of negative affect, a point not raised by the authors, as they focused on comparisons of smoking to neutral cues rather than comparisons of negative and positive affect.

Examining changes in emotional response to cues is important, but comparison of levels of negative and positive affect may provide different—and potentially equally important—information. For example, Sayette and Hufford (1995) found that participants manifested more positively valenced facial expressions than negative during initial cue exposure, and that more positive urge characteristics were self-generated during cigarette cue exposure compared to neutral cues, a relationship not found for negative urge characteristics (Sayette & Hufford, 1997). This study suggests that comparison of negative and positive affect might highlight the role of positive affect in cue-reactivity which is often obscured in other study designs. Moreover, a comparison of emotional responses may elucidate the relative contributions of positive and negative affect, both together and separately, on craving responses.

Highlighting the role of positive affect in cue-reactivity is only important if positive affect influences craving and/or behavior. Considering the robust relationship between craving and negative affect, it may be that “pure” positive affect has little role in the craving process. However, there may be benefit in modeling simultaneous positive and negative affect, dubbed “mixed emotions”(Larsen and McGraw, 2011, Larsen et al., 2001, Larsen et al., 2004, Larsen and Stastny, 2011). In basic emotion research, Larsen and colleagues have demonstrated that many people experience heightened positive and negative affect when confronted with a film that is both funny and tragic; when experiencing the celebration and fear associated with graduating from college; or when reacting to disappointing wins and relieving losses (Larsen et al., 2009, Larsen et al., 2001, Larsen et al., 2004).

The concept of mixed emotions calls into question the assumption that positive and negative affect must be mutually exclusive, an assumption that is common in basic emotion research (e.g., Russell, 2003) as well as implicitly endorsed in many of the cue-reactivity studies reviewed above. If negative affect and positive affect are opposites, and mutually exclusive, when negative affect goes up, positive affect must, by definition, go down. However, if positive and negative affect are independent and modeled simultaneously, we can establish if cues elicit relatively pure emotional responses or if cue-reactivity may be associated with a mixed emotional response.

The idea that craving might involve both positive and negative affect processes is certainly not new (Baker et al., 1987, Breiner et al., 1999, Kavanagh et al., 2005). However, to our knowledge, the association between craving and mixed emotion has not been explicitly tested. Nonetheless, several studies have yielded findings consistent with a mixed emotional perspective. Taylor et al. (2000) created a variety of imagery scripts that differed in valence and smoking urge, and examined how the scripts influenced positive affect, negative affect and craving. For positively valenced scripts, the inclusion of urge-related material decreased positive affect and increased negative affect compared to the no-urge scripts. However, for negatively valenced scripts, the inclusion of urge-related material resulted in lower negative affect and heightened positive affect compared to no-urge negative scripts. Thus, the addition of the urge component attenuated the effect of negative stimuli on self-reported affect. These results are consistent with a mixed emotional experience; the urge component elicited some positivity to the negative scripts, and some negativity to the positive scripts.

One recent study investigating ambivalence about smoking explicitly examined mixed emotional facial expressions during a cue reactivity paradigm. Among regular smokers who were 7 hours deprived, Griffin and Sayette (2008) measured positive and negative facial expressions while smokers were exposed to smoking and neutral cues. They found that 24% of participants exhibited both positive and negative facial expressions in response to smoking cues, where no mixed expressions (termed ‘ambivalent’ by the authors) were evident in response to neutral cues. Moreover, participants who displayed mixed expressions also evidenced elevated withdrawal symptoms and a greater interest in quitting compared to participants who did not display a mixed emotional response.

It is important to test the relationship between craving and mixed emotions for several reasons. A mixed emotional experience is qualitatively different, and appears to be theoretically less common than either a “pure” negative experience or a “pure” positive experience (Larsen, Hemenover, Norris, & Cacioppo, 2003). In other words, there is inherent conflict in a mixed emotional experience that may result in experiential discomfort. If craving elicits a mixed emotional experience, and subsequent cigarette smoking resolves the mixed feeling, simultaneous positive and negative reinforcement processes may be at work. Moreover, considering mixed emotional responses allows a role for positive affect in the craving process, which is theoretically suggested and may be obscured by considering positive and negative separately.

The current study was designed to extend cue-reactivity research in several ways. First, in addition to providing smoking and neutral cues, as is common in cue-reactivity studies (e.g., Bailey et al., 2010, Doran et al., 2008, Sayette and Hufford, 1995), pleasant and unpleasant affective cues were also included in order to better facilitate understanding of the craving-emotion relationship. Specifically, pleasant and unpleasant cues were included as additional comparison points to neutral cues to replicate recent findings that positive cues prompt heightened craving compared to negative and neutral cues (Muñoz et al., 2010, Robinson et al., 2011).

Second, we investigated the relationship between mixed emotion and craving using a measure of emotion developed from an emotion model that posits negative and positive affect as partially independent systems (Evaluative Space Model; Cacioppo and Bernston, 1994, Cacioppo and Berntson, 1999). We predicted that compared to the other valenced cue types, mixed emotions would be heightened in response to cigarette cues. We also predicted that mixed emotions would predict self-reported craving above and beyond mixed emotional responses to neutral cues.

Third, to attend to critiques that cue-reactivity research often neglects behavioral outcomes (Perkins, 2009, Rosenberg, 2009, Sayette et al., 2000), we hypothesized that craving in response to cigarette cues would predict risk-taking behavior. Moreover, we investigated these relationships in a sample of both regular smokers and tobacco chippers, guided by the assumption that the relationship between emotion and smoking might differ for smokers with different dependence and smoking frequency rates.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were recruited via online advertisements (Craigslist) in a large Midwestern city. Respondents over age 18 (N = 1600) were screened to determine eligibility. Inclusion criteria were: smoking at the same rate for two years, not currently trying to quit, no more than three quit attempts in the past two years with none in the last six months, no self-identified cardiovascular disease, not pregnant, and no regular use of other tobacco products. Eligible participants were invited to the

Participant characteristics

One hundred qualified participants completed the laboratory study. However, ten individuals who refused to smoke as part of the experiment as well as four others who had substantial missing data were excluded from further analyses, resulting in a final sample of 86 participants: 41 tobacco chippers (19 women, 22 men) and 45 regular smokers (21 females, 24 males). The ethnic breakdown was 66.3% Caucasian, 12.8% African American, 10.5% Latino/a, 4.7% Asian American, and an additional 5.8% that

Discussion

The central goals of the study were to (a) investigate emotion and craving response to a full set of valenced cues (pleasant/appetitive, unpleasant/aversive, neutral and cigarette), (b) model the relationship between simultaneous positive and negative affect or mixed emotional responses and craving for cigarettes, and (c) examine the effects of self-reported craving on subsequent behavior.

As expected, craving responses were stronger to cigarette cues than to neutral cues, replicating many past

Role of funding sources

This work was supported by an American Psychological Association Dissertation Award, a Society for the Science of Clinical Psychology Dissertation Award, and a University of Illinois at Chicago Provost award for graduate research given to the first author. The providers of these sources had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Contributors

Jennifer Veilleux designed the study with assistance from Jon Kassel. Megan Conrad provided data management and oversight of data collection. Jennifer Veilleux wrote the manuscript with editing done by Jon Kassel and Megan Conrad.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References (72)

  • T.H. Brandon et al.

    Affect, expectancies, urges and smoking: Do they conform to models of drug motivation and relapse?

    Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology

    (1996)
  • M.J. Breiner et al.

    Approaching avoidance: A step essential to the understanding of craving

    Alcohol Research & Health

    (1999)
  • J.T. Cacioppo et al.

    Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1994)
  • J.T. Cacioppo et al.

    The affect system: Architecture and operating characteristics

    Current directions in psychological science

    (1999)
  • B.L. Carter et al.

    A psychometric evaluation of cigarette stimuli used in a cue reactivity study

    Nicotine & Tobacco Research

    (2006)
  • B.L. Carter et al.

    Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research

    Addiction

    (1999)
  • B.L. Carter et al.

    The cue-availability paradigm: The effects of cigarette availability on cue reactivity in smokers

    Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology

    (2001)
  • Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention

    The international affective picture system (IAPS)

    (1995)
  • C.R.E. Coggins et al.

    Light and intermittent cigarette smokers: A review (1989–2009)

    Psychopharmacology

    (2009)
  • C.A. Conklin et al.

    The impact of imagining personalized versus standardized urge scenarios on cigarette craving and autonomic reactivity

    Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology

    (2001)
  • T.V. Cooper et al.

    Differences between intermittent and light daily smokers in a population of U.S. military recruits

    Nicotine & Tobacco Research

    (2010)
  • L.S. Cox et al.

    Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges in laboratory and clinical settings

    Nicotine & Tobacco Research

    (2001)
  • Cyders, M.A. (2008). Manipulation of positive emotion and its effects on negative outcomes of gambling behaviors and...
  • M.A. Cyders et al.

    Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: Positive and negative urgency

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2008)
  • M.A. Cyders et al.

    Integration of impulsivity and positive mood to predict risky behavior: Development and validation of a measure of positive urgency

    Psychological Assessment

    (2007)
  • J.P. Dempsey et al.

    Appetitive nature of drug cues re-confirmed with physiological measures and the potential role of stage of change

    Psychopharmacology

    (2007)
  • N. Doran et al.

    Cue-elicited negative affect in impulsive smokers

    Psychology of Addictive Behaviors

    (2008)
  • D.J. Drobes et al.

    Induction of smoking urge through imaginal and in vivo procedures: Physiological and self-report manifestations

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (1997)
  • M.S. Dunbar et al.

    Do smokers crave cigarettes in some smoking situations more than others? Situational correlates of craving when smoking

    Nicotine & Tobacco Research

    (2010)
  • P.A. Gable et al.

    The motivational dimensional model of affect: Implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorization

    Cognition & Emotion

    (2010)
  • K.M. Griffin et al.

    Facial reactions to smoking cues relate the ambivalence about smoking

    Psychology of Addictive Behaviors

    (2008)
  • B.L. Hankin et al.

    Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability-transactional stress theory

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2001)
  • S.J. Heishman et al.

    Prolonged duration of craving, mood, and automatic responses elicited by cues and images in smokers: Effects of tobacco deprivation and sex

    Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology

    (2010)
  • J. Hong et al.

    Feeling mixed but not torn: The moderating role of construal level in mixed emotions appeals

    Journal of Consumer Research

    (2010)
  • J.D. Kassel et al.

    The acute effects of nicotine on positive and negative affect in adolescent smokers

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2007)
  • D.J. Kavanagh et al.

    Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire

    Psychological Review

    (2005)
  • Cited by (17)

    • Associations between affect, craving, and smoking in Korean smokers: An ecological momentary assessment study

      2020, Addictive Behaviors Reports
      Citation Excerpt :

      In another meta-analysis, Heckman et al. (2015) reported that acute manipulations of negative affect also increased smoking assessed by latency to smoke and number of puffs, although the effect size was smaller less robust than that reported for craving. However, the relationship between positive affect and craving in Western smokers is less clear (Veilleux, Conrad, & Kassel, 2013). A meta-analysis of cue-provoked craving studies found positive affect inductions demonstrated inconsistent effects on craving (Heckman et al., 2013).

    • Varieties of mixed emotional experience

      2017, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
    • Adolescent's respiratory sinus arrhythmia is associated with smoking rate five years later

      2016, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Participants’ RSA was recorded before and after smoking. A more detailed description of laboratory procedures are published elsewhere (Conrad et al., 2015; Veilleux, Conrad, & Kassel, 2013). Demographic information, including race/ethnicity, education, gender were collected.

    • Mechanism of psychological distress-driven smoking addiction behavior

      2015, Journal of Business Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Disentangling the sources of psychological distress could also offer more precise insights into the mechanisms underlying the effect of psychological distress. In addition, future studies may extend this research by incorporating positive affect as well as negative affect to explain both regular smoking and addictive smoking, given that positive/pleasant emotion elicits urge to smoke more than negative emotion does (Veilleux, Conrad, & Kassel, 2013). In other words, urge to smoke can take two pathways (through negative and positive emotions).

    • Preliminary Effects of Progressive Muscle Relaxation on Cigarette Craving and Withdrawal Symptoms in Experienced Smokers in Acute Cigarette Abstinence: A Randomized Controlled Trial

      2015, Behavior Therapy
      Citation Excerpt :

      In healthy subjects, stress reduction or relaxation techniques reduce oxygen consumption, carbon dioxide production, respiratory rate, blood pressure, blood lactate levels, and heart rate—the so-called relaxation response (Beary, & Benson, 1974; Benson, Rosner, Marzetta, & Klemchuk, 1974). Previous studies showed that feelings (e.g., stress, negative or positive emotions) caused by environmental or internal factors could induce cravings for a substance (Ameringer, & Leventhal, 2010; Heckman et al., 2013; McKee et al., 2011; Veilleux, Conrad, & Kassel, 2013). We assumed that since nicotine or cigarette withdrawal symptoms are perceived as stressful by abstaining smokers—negative emotions including anxiety and restlessness are recognized as nicotine withdrawal symptoms (Javitz et al., 2012; Leventhal, Ameringer, Osborn, Zvolensky, & Langdon, 2013; West et al., 2006)—these stressful withdrawal symptoms would increase cravings for nicotine and therefore a relaxation technique might be helpful in reducing the stress response that may worsen the experience of craving.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text