Elsevier

Addictive Behaviors

Volume 31, Issue 8, August 2006, Pages 1383-1401
Addictive Behaviors

Sensation seeking and alcohol use: A meta-analytic review

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.11.004Get rights and content

Abstract

A meta-analysis was conducted to investigate the association between sensation seeking and alcohol use. Analysis of 61 studies revealed a small to moderate size, heterogeneous effect (mean weighted r = .263) between alcohol use and sensation seeking total scale scores. Analysis of the four sensation seeking components indicated that disinhibition was most strongly correlated with alcohol use (mean weighted r = .368). Categorical moderator analyses revealed that studies that did, versus did not, analyze covariates yielded smaller effect sizes. Continuous moderator analyses indicated that earlier published studies and higher percentages of male and white Caucasian participants were associated with stronger effect sizes between sensation seeking and alcohol use. Novel statistical methodologies for analyzing the association between sensation seeking and alcohol use were proposed and recommendations for future research were suggested.

Introduction

Sensation seeking is a personality trait that is characterized by the tendency to seek out varied and novel sensations and experiences. These experiences may include participation in risky physical activities (e.g., mountain climbing, bungee jumping, or skydiving), an attraction to novel political and philosophical ideologies, or participation in activities that are potentially addictive such as gambling and substance use. Sensation seeking is thought to be a normally distributed personality characteristic that is influenced by both biological and environmental factors (Zuckerman, 1994). In addition, sensation seeking is conceptualized as being a multidimensional, or multifaceted, personality construct (Zuckerman, 1979, Zuckerman, 1994) that is comprised of the following four components: thrill and adventure seeking, experience seeking, disinhibition, and boredom susceptibility. These four components are assessed by the Sensation Seeking Scale, or SSS (for reliability and validity information and for information bearing on the development of the instrument, see Zuckerman, 1979, Zuckerman, 1994). As regards to the SSS's factorial validity, numerous empirical studies conducted in the United States and abroad support the instrument's four-factor structure (see chapter two of Zuckerman, 1994, for a review. See also Loas et al., 2001, Roberti et al., 2003, Rowland and Franken, 1986).

The thrill and adventure seeking (TAS) component is characterized by a desire to engage in activities that involve speed or danger, such as downhill skiing or cliff diving. Whereas TAS is expressed through physical pursuits, a second dimension of sensation seeking, experience seeking (ES), reflects the need for novel personal or inner experiences. With this type of sensation seeking there is a preference for new and different experiences that might be achieved through travel to exotic destinations, interaction with people from different cultures, or learning about new philosophies. A third component of sensation seeking, disinhibition (DIS), is characterized by the expression of reduced social restraint. Individuals with this behavioral tendency are less constrained by societal norms and expectations and so they are more experimental with regard to their behavior. High disinhibitors often report having many sexual partners and they are more likely to both gamble and engage in illicit drug use (Zuckerman, 1994). The final dimension of sensation seeking is labeled boredom susceptibility (BS). BS is evident when an individual frequently reports distaste for anything routine or predictable. To counter their tendencies toward boredom, individuals high in BS often seek out new experiences and they seek out new people with whom to share such experiences. Perhaps not surprisingly, people high in this type of sensation seeking often have problems maintaining long-term personal relationships, but they do thrive in professions involving changing environments, such as aviation (Zuckerman, 1994).

Although numerous articles and book chapters have been written on the causes, correlates and consequences of sensation seeking, few topics have received as much attention as the association between sensation seeking and alcohol use (see, for example, Alterman et al., 1990, Brennan et al., 1986, Cherpitel, 1993, Donohew et al., 1999, Zuckerman and Kuhlman, 2000). Higher levels of sensation seeking have generally been shown to correlate with greater quantity and frequency of alcohol consumption (Zuckerman, 1994). Moreover, empirical research on treatment-seeking adults has found a negative relationship between sensation seeking and both age at first use of alcohol and age of onset of alcohol abuse (Ball, Carroll, & Rounsaville, 1994), suggesting that high sensation seekers first use and abuse alcohol at an earlier age than their lower scoring counterparts. Relative to other measures of personality, sensation seeking has been shown to be a particularly strong predictor of initial substance use and abuse in adolescence across a variety of drug use categories (Andrucci et al., 1989, Jaffe and Archer, 1987).

In light of these empirical research findings, the question naturally arises as to why it is that high sensation seeking is associated with greater alcohol use. The predominant theoretical explanation for this finding stems from research showing that sensation seeking is negatively correlated with platelet levels of monoamine oxidase, or MAO (see chapter 11 of Zuckerman, 1994, for a review). MAO regulates levels of monoamines, such as dopamine and norepinephrine, by breaking them down either after reuptake, or in the synaptic cleft (Nolte, 1993). A number of studies have shown that heavier drinkers have lower MAO levels than do lighter drinkers (Zuckerman, 1994). The negative correlation between sensation seeking and MAO is thought to be due, in large part, to the positive correlation between sensation seeking and dopamine (Netter and Rammsayer, 1991, Zuckerman, 1994). Elevated levels of dopamine might lead high sensation seekers to become heavier consumers of alcohol because (a) dopamine motivates appetitive reward seeking behavior, especially when the rewards are biologically (intrinsically) reinforcing (Zuckerman, 1994), and (b) alcohol and other drug use stimulate dopamine release in the central area of the brain's reward system (i.e., the ventral striatum and nucleus accumbens) (Koob and Le Moal, 1997, Robbins and Everitt, 1996). The released dopamine, in turn, is thought to be biologically rewarding thereby further reinforcing future substance use behavior (Wise, 1980). Additional evidence supporting the link between dopamine and drinking behavior comes from molecular genetics research showing a strong association between the D2 dopamine receptor (DRD2) polymorphism and both drinking quantity and severity of alcohol dependence (Connor, Young, Lawford, Ritchie, & Noble, 2002). Genetic support for the association between dopamine and sensation seeking comes from research showing an association between the Type 4 dopamine receptor gene polymorphism and novelty seeking (Keltikangas-Järvinen et al., 2003).

From a theoretical standpoint then it is certainly reasonable to predict a relationship between sensation seeking and alcohol use. To ascertain the extent of research activity on sensation seeking and drinking, a keyword search was conducted using the PsycINFO database (search conducted on 5-16-05). The search yielded 398 citations for the terms “sensation seeking and alcohol.” The citation count, in parentheses, for other sensation seeking–drug use combinations was as follows: marijuana (71), cigarettes (39), cocaine (44), heroin (15), stimulant (22), and caffeine (8) (the terms “smoking” and “opiate” yielded 88 and 15 citations, respectively). These citation data indicate that among studies examining sensation seeking and substance use, alcohol-related topics have received the lion's share of research attention. Furthermore, as noted above, the relevant studies on personality and drinking behavior indicate, with few exceptions, a positive association between sensation seeking and alcohol use. Moreover, of the various sensation seeking components, disinhibition appears to be the single strongest correlate of drinking behavior. However, despite the general consensus in the literature of a positive relationship between sensation seeking and drinking, there are several issues that remain unresolved.

First and perhaps foremost, given the wide range of methodological and data analytic differences across studies, it is difficult to discern the overall, or average, magnitude of the association between sensation seeking and alcohol use. To illustrate, there is a large degree of variability in sample sizes across studies, suggesting that some effect sizes are less reliable (have larger standard errors) than others. Furthermore, some of the smaller n studies examining the association between sensation seeking and alcohol use may have been underpowered (for a recent discussion of the causes of, and problems associated with, underpowered behavioral science research, see Maxwell, 2004). A second unresolved issue concerns the extent to which one or more variables might moderate the association between sensation seeking and alcohol use. In an attempt to address these concerns and limitations, a comprehensive quantitative synthesis of the sensation seeking and alcohol use literature was performed. The primary goal of the meta-analysis was to determine the mean and median effect sizes between sensation seeking (and each of the four components) and alcohol consumption. A second goal was to examine the extent to which various methodological and demographic characteristics moderated the strength of association between sensation seeking and alcohol use.

Section snippets

Literature search

Two strategies were employed to obtain relevant empirical research articles. First, keyword searches were conducted using three electronic databases: PsycINFO, Medline, and Conference Papers Index. Second, we perused the reference lists of key review articles and book chapters for additional relevant studies. Electronic search terms included alcohol, alcohol use, alcohol abuse, alcohol dependence, alcoholism, drinking, beer, wine, liquor, and ethanol crossed with sensation seeking, thrill

Results

The final pool of 61 articles included a total of 37,058 research participants. The mean sample size was 607.5 (SD = 822.9), the median was 321, and the sample sizes ranged from a low of 20 to a high of 4927. The study characteristics (e.g., design type, sample size, nature of participants, mean age, etc.), and the weighted effect sizes between alcohol use and sensation seeking for each study, are presented in Table 1. As these data indicate, the vast majority of studies were cross-sectional

Discussion

The objectives of the present meta-analysis were to ascertain the overall mean (and median) effect sizes between sensation seeking and alcohol use and to examine potential moderators of these effect size estimates. As regards to the overall strength of association, the mean weighted correlation between alcohol consumption and sensation seeking total scale scores (rw = .263) was small to moderate in magnitude. Of the four sensation seeking components, disinhibition evidenced the largest mean

References2 (91)

  • T.J. Johnson et al.

    Sensation seeking and drinking game participation in heavy-drinking college students

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2000)
  • B.A. Jonah et al.

    Sensation seeking, risky driving and behavioral adaptation

    Accident Analysis and Prevention

    (2001)
  • L. LaGrange et al.

    Alcohol consumption: Biochemical and personality correlates in a college student population

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1995)
  • M. Lejoyeux et al.

    Epidemiology of behavioral dependence: Literature review and results of original studies

    European Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • F. Liraud et al.

    Which temperamental characteristics are associated with substance use in subjects with psychotic and mood disorders?

    Psychiatry Research

    (2000)
  • C.A. Martin et al.

    Sensation seeking, puberty, and nicotine, alcohol, and marijuana use in adolescence

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

    (2002)
  • P. Netter et al.

    Reactivity to dopaminergic drugs and aggression-related personality traits

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (1991)
  • C.P.L. Ripa et al.

    A Danish version of the sensation seeking scale and its relation to a broad spectrum of behavioral and psychological characteristics

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2001)
  • T.W. Robbins et al.

    Neurobehavioral mechanisms of reward and motivation

    Current Opinions in Neurobiology

    (1996)
  • G.L. Rowland et al.

    The four dimensions of sensation seeking: A confirmatory factor analysis

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (1986)
  • T.R. Simon et al.

    Sensation seeking and drug use among high risk Latino and Anglo adolescents

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (1994)
  • J.A. Webb et al.

    Risk factors and their relation to initiation of alcohol use among early adolescents

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

    (1991)
  • R.A. Wise

    Actions of drugs of abuse on the brain reward systems

    Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior

    (1980)
  • R.J. Alston

    Sensation seeking as a psychological trait of drug abuse among persons with spinal cord injury

    Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin

    (1994)
  • G.L. Andrucci et al.

    The relationship of MMPI and sensation seeking scales to adolescent drug use

    Journal of Personality Assessment

    (1989)
  • K. Baker et al.

    Self-esteem, alcoholism, sensation seeking, gpa, and differential aptitude test scores of high school students in an honor society

    Psychological Reports

    (1991)
  • J.R. Baker et al.

    Moderating effect of gender on the relationship between sensation seeking-impulsivity and substance use in adolescents

    Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse

    (2002)
  • S.A. Ball et al.

    Sensation seeking, substance abuse, and psychopathology in treatment-seeking and community cocaine abusers

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1994)
  • K.A. Bollen

    Structural equations with latent variables

    (1989)
  • A.F. Brennan et al.

    Alcohol use and abuse in college students: A review of individual and personality correlates

    International Journal of the Addictions

    (1986)
  • C.J. Cherpitel

    Alcohol, injury, and risk-taking behavior: Data from a national sample

    Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research

    (1993)
  • A.M. Crawford et al.

    Parallel developmental trajectories of sensation seeking and regular substance use in adolescents

    Psychology of Addictive Behaviors

    (2003)
  • D.M. Dick et al.

    Testing between-family associations in within-family comparisons

    Psychological Science

    (2000)
  • R.L. Donohew et al.

    Sensation seeking and drug use by adolescents and their friends: Models for marijuana and alcohol

    Journal of Studies on Alcohol

    (1999)
  • R.A. Fisher

    Statistical methods for research workers

    (1928)
  • M.A. Forney et al.

    A profile and prediction study of problem drinking among first-year medical students

    The International Journal of the Addictions

    (1988)
  • G. Forsyth et al.

    Personality and situation as determinants of desire to drink in young adults

    The International Journal of the Addictions

    (1987)
  • L.F. Forthun et al.

    Religiosity, sensation seeking, and alcohol/drug use in denominational and gender contexts

    Journal of Drug Issues

    (1999)
  • M.J. Henderson et al.

    Covariance structure models of expectancy

    Journal of Studies on Alcohol

    (1994)
  • J.B. Hittner

    Novel methods for analyzing multifaceted personality scales: Sense of coherence and depression as an example

    The Journal of Psychology

    (2000)
  • J.B. Hittner et al.

    A Monte Carlo evaluation of tests for comparing dependent correlations

    Journal of General Psychology

    (2003)
  • J.G. Hull et al.

    A general approach to testing multifaceted personality constructs

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1991)
  • Y. Hur et al.

    The genetic correlation between impulsivity and sensation seeking traits

    Behavior Genetics

    (1997)
  • L.T. Jaffe et al.

    The prediction of drug use among college students from MMPI, MCMI, and sensation seeking scales

    Journal of Personality Assessment

    (1987)
  • P.B. Johnson

    Personality correlates of heavy and light drinking female college students

    Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education

    (1989)
  • Cited by (259)

    • Alcohol culture and corporate risk-taking

      2024, Research in International Business and Finance
    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Portions of this research were previously presented at the annual meeting of the American Association of Behavioral and Social Sciences, Las Vegas, NV (February, 2005), and the Society for Southeastern Social Psychologists Conference, Clemson, SC (November, 2004).

    2

    References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

    View full text