Elsevier

Acta Psychologica

Volume 164, February 2016, Pages 103-111
Acta Psychologica

The role of visual similarity and memory in body model distortions

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.12.013Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We examine the role of visual similarity and memory in body model distortions.

  • Hand and object representations were compared in localization and memory tasks.

  • Memory effects cannot account for the amount of distortions measured on the hand.

  • Distortions on objects do not increase with hand similarity.

  • The unique magnitude of hand distortions supports the existence of a body model.

Abstract

Several studies have shown that the perception of one's own hand size is distorted in proprioceptive localization tasks. It has been suggested that those distortions mirror somatosensory anisotropies. Recent research suggests that non-corporeal items also show some spatial distortions. In order to investigate the psychological processes underlying the localization task, we investigated the influences of visual similarity and memory on distortions observed on corporeal and non-corporeal items. In experiment 1, participants indicated the location of landmarks on: their own hand, a rubber hand (rated as most similar to the real hand), and a rake (rated as least similar to the real hand). Results show no significant differences between rake and rubber hand distortions but both items were significantly less distorted than the hand. Experiments 2 and 3 explored the role of memory in spatial distance judgments of the hand, the rake and the rubber hand. Spatial representations of items measured in experiments 2 and 3 were also distorted but showed the tendency to be smaller than in localization tasks. While memory and visual similarity seem to contribute to explain qualitative similarities in distortions between the hand and non-corporeal items, those factors cannot explain the larger magnitude observed in hand distortions.

Introduction

There is probably no more familiar object to us than our own body. This might give rise to the impression that we know our body better than anything else. This impression partly comes from the fact that we receive constant and immediate sensory information about our body. A single glance at one's hand and we know its location in space as well as its relative proportions with other limbs (e.g., the hand is smaller than the arm). Consequently, it seems natural to assume that we have an accurate perception of the size and shape of our body and its parts. However, multiple studies indicate the presence of systematic distortions in the perception of bodily proportions (Linkenauger et al., 2015, Longo and Haggard, 2010, Longo and Haggard, 2011, Longo and Haggard, 2012, Saulton et al., 2015). Those distortions were demonstrated in visual estimations tasks (Linkenauger et al., 2015, Longo and Haggard, 2012) as well as in tactile and localization tasks (Longo and Haggard, 2010, Longo and Haggard, 2011). In this study, we are particularly interested in better understanding the origin of the distortions measured in localization tasks (Longo & Haggard, 2010).

Localizing one's body in space is important for perception and action (Frith, Blakemore, & Wolpert, 2000). For instance, one needs to know the location of one's hand in order to grasp objects (Frith et al., 2000). Research suggests that localization judgments related to our body parts are based on the combination of proprioceptive signals (e.g. joint angles) and stored representation of body size and shape (van Beers et al., 1998, Longo and Haggard, 2010, Soechting, 1982). This stored representation of the body metric properties, referred to as the body model, was measured in a localization task for the hand (Longo & Haggard, 2010). Participants were asked to point towards the felt location of their occluded finger tips and knuckles. By analyzing the spatial configuration of the felt locations of the finger tips and knuckles, implicit maps of hand shape were created. Those maps showed large distortions of hand shape. This pattern of distortion was characterized by an overestimation of hand width and an underestimation of finger length.

Interestingly, distortions of hand shape measured in localization tasks matched those found in tactile size perception of the hand (Linkenauger et al., 2015, Longo and Haggard, 2011, Weber, 1996). Hand distortions measured in localization tasks were consistent with anisotropies characterizing the hand's tactile acuity and receptive field geometry (Longo and Haggard, 2010, Longo and Haggard, 2011). Hand distortions were therefore interpreted as retaining “vestigial traces of the primary somatosensory homunculus of Penfield” (p. 11729, Longo & Haggard, 2010).

However, there is no direct evidence that hand distortions in the localization task are due to somatosensation. Particularly, the localization task does not involve tactile perception, as the hand is not touched during the experimentation (see method in, Longo and Haggard, 2010, Saulton et al., 2015). As such, there may be no direct link between anisotropic tactile sensitivity of the hand and hand shape distortions measured in localization tasks.

Indeed, localization tasks distortions were not limited to the hand and appeared to generalize onto certain types of objects, particularly in the case of a rake (Saulton et al., 2015) Distortions measured on the rake item were more similar to the one found on the participant's hand than on other objects depicting square and rectangular shapes. Although the amount of distortion was significantly smaller on the rake than on the hand, it was also characterized by an overestimation of the width axis compared to a large underestimation of the length. The purpose of the present paper is to better understand why distortions would be more similar across a rake and a hand than across a hand and other geometrical objects. We will explore both body and non-body related factors that might account for these results.

We explored whether an item's visual similarity to a real hand was behind the greater performance similarity between the hand and the rake. Due to structural similitudes between the hand and the rake (e.g. five fingers/five tines), it could be that participants partly matched the representation of their hand onto the stored spatial representation of the rake. Hand shapes are more familiar to participants than tools. Hence, matching strategies could be used in localization task as an attempt to improve one's performance in the localization task. If this is the case, an object with greater visual similarity to a real hand (e.g. a rubber hand) might depict distortions that are closer to the hand than the rake. This idea would be in line with research on embodiment showing that objects can be experienced as part of one's body (i.e. as embodied) when they share important structural and visual information about the body part (Bertamini and O'Sullivan, 2014, Holmes et al., 2006, Tsakiris et al., 2010, Tsakiris and Haggard, 2005). Studies on the rubber hand illusion suggest that the degree to which fake body parts (rubber hand and non-biological mechanical hand) can be embodied depends on the similarity between the actual body part and the tested stimulus. For instance, embodiment of a rubber hand is facilitated and obtained to a larger degree compared to a non-biological hand made of wires (Bertamini & O'Sullivan, 2014). Although embodiment mechanisms are unlikely to occur in the localization task (no visuo-tactile stimulation applied onto the participant's hand and the tested stimulus), one cannot exclude the possibility that greater visual similarity between an item and a real hand contribute to an increase in localization task distortions. This aspect was investigated in experiment 1 by comparing participants' estimates of landmarks located on a rubber hand, a rake and the participants' hand in a localization task.

Alternatively, the similarity in localization task distortions between the hand and rake might be explained by non-body specific factors. Previous work suggests the presence of viewer-centered biases and immediate vision on hand distortions in localization tasks (Longo, 2014, Saulton et al., 2015). In line with these ideas, people might also partially rely on a general form of memory (e.g. spatial memory) that is not directly related to proprioception. Overall, memory distortions have been observed in multiple studies, from tasks involving the recollection of stories or experienced events (Bartlett, 1932, Nourkova et al., 2004) to psychophysical experiments measuring object size perception, localization and distance estimations on maps and figures (Cooper et al., 2012, Huttenlocher et al., 2004, Tversky, 1981, Tversky, 1992, Tversky and Schiano, 1989). For instance, distances stored in memory between entities of the same categories (cities on map) are perceived relatively smaller compared to distances between entities of different categories (Tversky, 1992). Semantically, fingers often constitute a separate body part category (Enfield, Majid, & Van Staden, 2006). Hence, memory biases related to finger categorization could explain why underestimation of finger length compared to hand width were found in localization tasks (Longo et al., 2015, Mattioni and Longo, 2014). To assess whether memory of distances between landmarks can create the distortions measured on items in the localization task, we ran a second experiment. In experiment 2, we asked participants to indicate on a line, the memorized distance between landmarks marking the finger/branches length and width of the hand, the rake and the rubber hand. We compared the ratio of length over width distortions obtained in this distance memory task (experiment 2) with the same length to width ratio calculated in localization task (experiment 1) for the same items.

In order to investigate whether the distortions measured on the participant's hand in the distance memory task can be behaviorally dissociated from distortions coming from the somatosensory feeling associated with one's own hand, we ran a third experiment. In experiment 3 participants indicated on a line, both the memorized and the felt distance between landmarks on their hand. Different results between the felt and memorized distance conditions of experiment 3 would favor the hypothesis that memory information about hand parts can be dissociated from information related to the somatosensory feelings associated to the hand.

Section snippets

Experiment 1

In experiment 1, we investigated the extent to which the similarities between the item and the participant's hand modulate the distortion measured in the localization task. In order to measure the contribution of visual similarity on the items' distortions, it is important to choose stimuli that gradually increase in visual similarity with a hand: a rake which only had a similar structure to a real hand; a rubber hand which had the structure and the visual configuration/form of a real hand and

Experiment 2

In experiment 2, we want to know whether directly memorizing the relative distance between landmarks located on the participant's hand, a rubber hand and a rake can retain distortions typically associated with localization judgment tasks. In the localization task, participants had to point on a screen towards the felt location of predefined landmarks on their hand (absolute landmark location). To decrease the participants' reliance on proprioception during body estimates, we used a memory task

Experiment 3

We performed a third experiment in which we asked two groups of participants to assess the distance between landmarks on their hand either by relying on their memory (stored visual information about hand parts) or by feeling (somatosensory information about hand parts) the distance (Longo & Haggard, 2012). The procedure was the same as in experiment 2 except for the looking time which was matched to the one used in the localization task (15 s). If participants predominantly relied on

General discussion

The goal of the study was to better understand the psychological processes underlying localization task results for corporeal and non-corporeal items. Specifically, we assessed the plausibility of visual similarity (experiment 1) and memory (experiments 2 and 3) as the driving force behind the distortions in the localization task.

Conclusion

In the study, we investigated some psychological processes underlying localization task distortions measured on corporeal and non-corporeal items. We have shown that visual similarity and memory factors are unlikely to fully explain localization task distortions. Hence, other factors related to body perception like viewer dependent representation (Künnapas, 1958), holistic perception (Tanaka & Farah, 1993), distorted conceptual knowledge of the hand (Longo, 2015) and somatosensory processing (

Acknowledgments

Matthew Longo was supported by European Research Council grant ERC-2013-StG-336050 under the FP7. Part of Heinrich Bülthoff's research was supported by the Brain Korea 21 PLUS Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea funded by the Ministry of Education. Aurelie Saulton was partly supported by the CIN Pool Project 2014-03: Investigating Body Representation Distortions in Patient Populations using Biometric Self-Avatars in Virtual Reality. We thank Trevor Dodds for useful

References (35)

  • B. Tversky

    Distortions in memory for maps

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1981)
  • B. Tversky

    Distortions in cognitive maps

    Geoforum

    (1992)
  • R.A. Abrams et al.

    Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization

    Perception & Psychophysics

    (1990)
  • F. Bartlett

    Remembering: An experimental and social study

    (1932)
  • F.W. Finger et al.

    The illustration of the horizontal–vertical illusion

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1947)
  • C.D. Frith et al.

    Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action

    Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences

    (2000)
  • K. Hamburger et al.

    Analysis of individual variations in the classical horizontal–vertical illusion

    Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics

    (2010)
  • Cited by (34)

    • Distortion of mental body representations

      2022, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Subsequent studies have suggested that more general factors may also contribute [69–74]. For example, similar distortions occur when participants localize landmarks on non-body objects based on visual memory [69,71]. Other studies report systematic conceptual misunderstanding of the location of landmarks such as knuckles, which could (at least partly) explain distortions such as underestimation of finger length [70,72,74].

    • Representation and perception of the body in space

      2021, Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience: Second Edition
    • How ageing shapes body and space representations: A comparison study between healthy young and older adults

      2021, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      This common result on both limbs suggests that the distortions reported in older participants at the BL task are not related to manual dominance or any cognitive lateralized mechanism. Another factor recently proposed to explain perceived distortions in body metrics is related to general conceptual distortions linked to mistaken beliefs or visual memory of the location of different landmarks (Longo et al., 2015; Saulton et al., 2016). Although it is not possible to discard this hypothesis at the moment, this proposal is unlikely in explaining the age-related effects for at least two reasons.

    • Metric biases in body representation extend to objects

      2021, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Longo and colleagues (2015) found that the horizontal dimension of a fake hand was less overestimated compared to the own hand in a localization task. On the contrary, Saulton et al. (2016) observed no differences between the representation of a fake and the subject's own hand in a size estimation task, where participants observed the target for 40 s, remembered their dimensions and provided their response in a paper-and-pencil task. The same study however reported increased distortions for the subject's hand compared to the fake hand in a different group of subjects performing the localization task.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text