Elsevier

Acta Psychologica

Volume 145, January 2014, Pages 54-64
Acta Psychologica

When the mind wanders: Age-related differences between young and older adults

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.10.016Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We assessed age-related differences in young and older adults' mind wandering.

  • Using the probe-caught method older adults reported fewer mind wandering.

  • Behavioral indices showed that old–old adults were more disengaged from the task.

  • Mind wandering is explained by age and working memory.

Abstract

Interest in mind wandering (MW) has grown in recent years, but few studies have assessed this phenomenon in older adults. The aim of this study was to assess age-related differences between young, young–old and old–old adults in MW using two versions of the sustained attention to response task (SART), one perceptual and one semantic. Different indicators were examined (i.e., reported MW episodes and behavioral indices of MW such as response time latency and variability, incorrect response and omission errors). The relationship between MW, certain basic mechanisms of cognition (working memory, inhibition and processing speed), cognitive failures and intrusive thoughts in everyday life was also explored. Findings in both versions of the SART indicated that older adults reported a lower frequency of MW episodes than young adults, but some of the behavioral indices of MW (response time variability, incorrect response and omission errors) were higher in old–old adults. This seems to suggest that MW becomes less frequent with aging, but more pervasive and detrimental to performance. Our results also indicated that the role of age and cognitive mechanisms in explaining MW depends on the demands of the SART task considered.

Introduction

Mind wandering (MW) can be defined as a shift of attention from the environmental context to stimuli and mental representations associated with personal thoughts or ongoing activities, i.e., task-unrelated thoughts (TUTs) (Antrobus et al., 1966, Giambra, 1995, Smallwood et al., 2003). Although this phenomenon is very common (Kane et al., 2007) and can be useful in some circumstances (Baird et al., 2011, Baird et al., 2012), it is often unintentional and leads to a less accurate information encoding with consequent cognitive failures and related psychological stress (Schupak and Rosenthal, 2009, Smallwood et al., 2006). Hence the interest in MW in numerous studies on different age groups (healthy younger and older adults), as well as in specific populations, including adults with ADHD (Giambra, 1993) and people with depression (e.g., Smallwood, O'Connor, Sudberry, & Obonsawin, 2007).

It is now well documented that aging coincides with a decline in some basic cognitive mechanisms, such as working memory, inhibition and processing speed (e.g., Craik and Salthouse, 2008, Hasher and Zacks, 1988). This decline also explains age-related differences in many cognitive domains and everyday life abilities (e.g., Borella, Ghisletta, & de Ribaupierre, 2011).

Recent theories on MW claim that executive control lies behind the MW phenomenon (McVay & Kane, 2009). As older adults generally have more limited executive control resources than young adults, i.e., less efficient inhibitory mechanisms (attentional control), and a weak working memory performance (Hasher & Zacks, 1988), we might expect them to experience more MW, and report more TUTs than young adults. Instead, an apparently paradoxical decrease in TUT frequency with aging has been reported in various studies and using various models. In particular, Giambra, 1973, Giambra, 1993, Giambra, 2000 used self-report questionnaires to investigate daydreaming, i.e., task-unrelated image and thought intrusions, finding that their frequency decreased with aging in both longitudinal (Giambra, 2000) and cross-sectional comparisons (Giambra, 1973, Giambra, 1993). Giambra (1989) also obtained much the same results using more objective methods, which were not sensitive to participants' beliefs and therefore unaffected by any biases (such as those induced by questionnaires). Giambra used a vigilance task (in which participants responded to rare targets) in a retrospective correlational study (1989) that involved 5 experiments (conducted from 1977 to 1980). The results confirmed that TUTs did not increase with aging. In particular, four of the five studies on the frequency of TUTs reported that old–old adults (over 70 years of age) had fewer TUTs than young or middle-aged adults; and three of the five studies also identified a lower frequency of TUTs in young–old (60- to 70-year-olds) than in younger adults. A negative correlation between age and TUTs across the lifespan also emerged in vigilance and reading tasks of variable difficulty (Grodsky & Giambra, 1990–1991). Giambra attributed the results concerning the decrease in TUTs (incompatible with the inhibitory hypothesis [Hasher & Zacks, 1988]) to various factors, such as age-related memory decline, or an age-dependent reduction in nonconscious information processing due to older people having less “unfinished business”, or fewer matters of concern, whereas young adults can devote their attention to MW as well because of their greater attentional capacity (Giambra, 1989).

Giambra's results (Giambra, 1989, Giambra, 1993) might also be due, however, to the introspective procedure used – in some experiments participants were asked to report when their mind was wandering during a task (self-caught method) – without taking into account the age-related differences in people's ability to monitor their internal states, and thus report on their TUTs (Einstein & McDaniel, 1997). To test this hypothesis, Einstein and McDaniel (1997) used a more objective and reliable performance-based measure, assessing the frequency of MW on the basis of participants' performance when they were occasionally interrupted while recalling long lists of words: if a participant recalled significantly fewer words than the average, this was interpreted as being due to MW, which led to their failure to encode the stimuli. The results of this study showed that older adults did not differ significantly from young people in this performance-based measure, and the authors concluded that there was no evidence of age-related differences in MW. Parks, Klinger, and Perlmutter (1988–89) came to a similar conclusion when they used a thought sampling procedure during the performance of more or less difficult tasks. They found a higher frequency of evaluative thoughts focusing on steps towards a goal and of attention-control utterances in older than in younger adults, regardless of the task's difficulty. Finally, Jackson and Balota (2012) recently investigated MW in younger and older adults using more recently developed experimental models. The authors presented different versions of the SART (a go/no-go task in which participants had to inhibit a habitual response), in which they also considered response latency as a possible performance-based indicator of MW, and a more demanding reading comprehension task. They expected to find the same level of MW in young and older adults in the more demanding test (the reading comprehension). Instead, they found that older adults reported less MW than their younger counterparts in both the SARTs and the reading comprehension task. The manipulation of the tasks' presentation had a different influence on the frequency of MW episodes reported by younger and older adults, however. In the first two experiments, the young adults reported four times as many MW episodes as the older adults; in the others, the young adults reported only twice as many MW episodes as the older adults (particularly in the more demanding tasks, i.e., the reading comprehension and a slower, longer version of the SART). Older adults also showed a disproportionate post-error slowing in their completion of the SARTs, probably because they found it more difficult to re-engage in the task (possibly due to a greater concern about their performance). The authors suggested that this pattern of results might reflect a greater engagement of older adults in the task (supported by personality measures indicating greater conscientiousness in this age group than in the young adults) that could lead to less MW and more self-evaluation thoughts after errors and that would explain the age-related differences in post-error latency.

Taken together, the above studies indicate either a similar frequency of MW being reported by young and older adults or (in the majority of the studies) a decreasing frequency of MW with aging; regardless of older adults' decline in inhibitory mechanisms (or more generally in executive control), the ability to maintain task goals or context over time could make it difficult to control intrusive thoughts (e.g., Hasher and Zacks, 1988, Balota et al., 1992).

It is noteworthy that it seems difficult to predict findings concerning age-related differences in MW (between young and older adults) on the strength of the two main hypotheses proposed in the literature (based mainly on evidence of this phenomenon in younger adults), i.e., the “decoupling” hypothesis (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006), and the “control failure × current concerns” hypothesis (McVay, Kane, & Kwapil, 2009). The decoupling hypothesis considers MW as a spontaneous process involving a state in which attention becomes coupled to an internal process and decoupled from the external information (e.g., Barron et al., 2011, Smallwood, 2010); accordingly, the frequency of MW would be modulated by the amount of resources required by the task and the amount of resources that the person possesses (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). Thus, older adults' more limited cognitive resources (Craik & Salthouse, 2008) would result in less frequent MW. The results that this hypothesis would lead us to expect were not seen in all of the above-cited studies (i.e., two of the five studies conducted by Einstein & McDaniel (1997), Giambra (1989) and Parks et al. (1988–89)). Nor are the findings reported by Jackson and Balota (2012) consistent with a similar decrease in the frequency of MW episodes that younger and older adults reported with increasingly difficult tasks, as predicted by this decoupling hypothesis.

It seems also difficult to fully explain the above-mentioned results from the “control failure × current concerns” perspective (e.g., McVay & Kane, 2010), according to which TUTs are automatically generated in response to environmental cues, current concerns and personal goals. They enter the sphere of our awareness as a result of an attentional control failure, disrupting goal maintenance processes, so MW would presumably be more common in people with a poor working memory capacity (McVay & Kane, 2009), such as older adults, who may also have difficulty in modulating their MW in relation to the resources demanded by the task. But, according to these authors, it is also important to consider both the amount of thoughts automatically generated and the category of thoughts reportedly involved. In fact, older adults may have fewer current concerns and personal goals, and this would lead to less MW, as Giambra (1989) suggested, but may report more thoughts about their performance, i.e., concern-related thoughts (McVay, Meier, Touron, & Kane, 2013).

We could therefore conclude that it is still not clear how some of the cognitive mechanisms (i.e., working memory and inhibition) evoked by the two different hypotheses in the literature contribute to explaining MW in young and older adults. Likewise, it is therefore still hard to say how a task's complexity (in terms of the demands of the task) modulates MW. In fact, it has been well documented that the frequency of MW decreases for more complex tasks in younger adults (see Smallwood & Schooler, 2006), but this is not the case for older adults. Jackson and Balota (2012) tried to shed light on this aspect, and their experiments showed the different influence of task manipulation on MW frequency as a function of age. But their findings cannot be used to draw any final conclusions because they used a between-groups design and several variables (not only the difficulty of the tasks) were manipulated across the experiments, including the type of task, the rate of presentation of the stimuli, and the duration of the task.

Finally, it is important to make the point that an aspect of the issue has been overlooked, i.e., that the different MW patterns described in the literature may also depend on the age range of the older adults considered, given the difference in the extent of cognitive decline between the young-old (65–74 year-olds) and the old-old (75–85 year-olds), which is more accentuated in the latter group (e.g., Baltes, 1987, Borella et al., 2008). The age brackets of older adults used in previous studies may have masked some of the differences in MW patterns with aging: Giambra divided his sample into two age groups (60–69, 70–89); Einstein and McDaniel (1997) studied a sample of young–old adults (60–76, M = 65.6), and Jackson and Balota (2012) considered elderly adults as a homogeneous group across experiments (their mean age ranged between 75.8 and 77.3 years).

Aiming to shed further light on this complex phenomenon, the present study explored the age-related differences in MW, comparing young, young-old and old–old adults, by: i) manipulating the demands of the tasks, presenting different versions of a SART; and ii) directly assessing the relationship between MW and certain cognitive mechanisms (working memory, inhibition and processing speed) using different versions of the SART. A multivariate design was adopted in which all the tasks considered were administered to all the participants.

The SART was chosen because it is the most commonly used paradigm for assessing MW. Concerning our first goal, to examine whether MW can be explained by the demands of a task (in terms of the cognitive resources involved), two versions of the SART were developed, one perceptual and one semantic. The semantic SART has the same structure as the perceptual one, but it involves participants simultaneously in two activities, i.e., encoding stimuli and detecting targets. Such a manipulation has been shown to prompt a different involvement of people's cognitive resources (see Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). In particular, less MW reportedly coincided with the more demanding SART in a sample of younger adults (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). We newly explored whether the greater resources needed for the semantic SART than for the perceptual version would reduce the frequency of MW in older adults too, a hypothesis that would be consistent with the decoupling hypothesis (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). To our knowledge at least, no studies conducted so far have examined this issue in both young and older adults using a multivariate design.

The frequency of MW was assessed using the probe-caught method. This is a widely-adopted method particularly useful in the case of older adults, who may be scarcely aware of their own mental processes because they do not have to constantly monitor their thoughts. The content of participants' MW episodes was also classified to obtain additional information on the source of these thoughts as a function of age and see whether there were any age-related differences in people's susceptibility to thoughts about personal goals and concerns, or to external cues, for instance. We thus distinguished between personal thoughts and daydreams (SITUTs), task-related interferences (TRIs) and external distractions (EDs) (see Stawarczyk, Majerus, Maj, Van Der Linden, & D'Argembeau, 2011). Using this classification, we assessed the generality vs. specificity of MW content. In particular, we expected to find: i) an age-related decrease in SITUTs, as predicted by Giambra (1989) and consistent with the two hypotheses in the literature on MW; and ii) an age-related increase in intrusive thoughts due to contextual cues (EDs and TRIs) in older adults as a result of inhibitory control failures and a greater focus on the smooth completion of the task (Jackson and Balota, 2012, Parks et al., 1988–89), which would be consistent with the ‘control failure × current concerns’ view of MW (e.g., McVay & Kane, 2010).

Given that older adults may have more difficulty in monitoring their thoughts, as Einstein and McDaniel (1997) suggested, the behavioral indicators of MW (i.e., response time latency and variability, incorrect responses and omission errors) usually assessed in younger populations (see Cheyne, Solman, Carriere, & Smildek, 2009) were also considered. Although these indices are sensitive to other factors (e.g., contextual distractions) as well as to MW, they can give us an important indication of the level of a participant's disengagement from a task. For instance, omission errors can be interpreted as being due to a deep level of MW because they have been found correlated with zoning-out episodes (i.e., MW without awareness) (Cheyne et al., 2009). If what Einstein and McDaniel said is true, we should see discrepancies between the reported MW episodes and the behavioral indicators: with increasing age, we might expect fewer reported MW episodes to coincide with a worse performance in the task and: a) more incorrect responses and omissions for non-target stimuli; b) pre-error speeding and post-error slowing (as found by Jackson & Balota, 2012); and c) a variance in the response times (RT CV). All these indicators would relate to more frequent, less conscious MW episodes.

Regarding the second goal of our study, we tried to shed more light on the relationship between the MW phenomenon, considered as a whole, and cognitive resources, using classical measures of working memory capacity (the Categorization Working Memory Span test, Borella et al., 2008), inhibitory efficacy (intrusion errors committed in the working memory task, Robert, Borella, Fagot, Lecerf, & de Ribaupierre, 2009), and processing speed (the pattern comparison test, Salthouse & Babcock, 1991). To our knowledge, no previous studies have investigated the joint role of these resources in explaining MW and individual and age-related differences in this phenomenon. According to the above-discussed hypotheses, we might expect either a decrease in MW with declining cognitive resources in aging, in agreement with the decoupling hypothesis, or else an increase in MW with decreasingly efficient inhibitory mechanisms and working memory (McVay & Kane, 2009). The present study should clarify this issue. Moreover, we expected cognitive resources to play a different part on the two versions of the SART considered, their relationship being stronger with the semantic SART, which demands more cognitive resources, than with the easier perceptual SART.

Finally, the relationship between MW and intrusive thoughts and cognitive failures due to inattention in everyday life was assessed with the aid of a new ad hoc questionnaire, which specifically indicated errors due to inattention (and not only to memory, as is typically the case in questionnaires on cognitive failure in everyday life). In particular, by means of this measure we wanted to see whether or not there was a sort of congruence between subjective (questionnaire) and more objective (performance-based, response latency, etc.) measures of MW in aging, as highlighted in younger adults after assessing self-reported everyday attention failures (Cheyne, Carriere, & Smildek, 2006), and mindfulness (Cheyne et al., 2006, Cheyne et al., 2009).

Section snippets

Participants

Our sample consisted of twenty young adults (20–30 years old), 20 young–old adults (65–74 years old), and 19 old–old adults1 (75–85 years old), who volunteered to take part in the study and were recruited by word of mouth. They were all native Italian speakers, and healthy. We ruled out anyone meeting any of the “exclusion criteria” proposed by Crook et al. (1986) — i.e. a history of head trauma, any neurological or psychiatric illness, a history of brain fever,

Results

First, ANOVA were carried out on the MW questionnaire, the working memory task, the proportion of intrusion errors in the CWMS (inhibitory measure), and the processing speed, with group as the independent variable. The results are summarized in Table 1. Then the results of the SARTs were analyzed (see below).

Discussion and conclusion

MW is a phenomenon that has recently received considerable attention, but few studies have assessed whether and how it changes with age, also in relation to the demands of a given task. To the best of our knowledge, none of the studies conducted to date have considered the relationship between MW and cognitive resources (working memory, inhibition, processing speed) in aging. We therefore newly investigated the age-related differences in MW between young, young–old and old–old individuals using

References (46)

  • J. Smallwood et al.

    Task-unrelated thought: The role of distributed processing

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2003)
  • J. Smallwood et al.

    Encoding during the attentional lapse: Accuracy of encoding during the semantic SART

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2006)
  • D. Stawarczyk et al.

    Mind-wandering: Phenomenology and function as assessed with a novel experience sampling method

    Acta Psychologica

    (2011)
  • J.S. Antrobus et al.

    Studies in the stream of consciousness: Experimental suppression of spontaneous cognitive process

    Perceptual and Motor Skills

    (1966)
  • B. Baird et al.

    Mind-wandering facilitates creative incubation

    (2012)
  • D.A. Balota et al.

    Automatic and attentional priming in young and older adults: Reevaluation of the two process model

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1992)
  • P.B. Baltes

    Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline

    Developmental Psychology

    (1987)
  • E. Barron et al.

    Absorbed in thought: The effect of mind wandering on the processing of relevant and irrelevant events

    Psychological Science

    (2011)
  • E. Borella et al.

    Working memory, control of interference and everyday experience of thought interference: When age makes the difference

    Aging Clinical and Experimental Research

    (2007)
  • E. Borella et al.

    Age differences in text processing: The role of working memory, inhibition and processing speed

    Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences

    (2011)
  • B. Carretti et al.

    Age differences in proactive interference in verbal and visuo-spatial working memory

    Journal of Cognitive Psychology

    (2011)
  • L.L. Carstensen et al.

    Emotion in the second half of life

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (1998)
  • J.A. Cheyne et al.

    Anatomy of an error: A bidirectional state of task engagement/disengagement and attention-related errors

    Cognition

    (2009)
  • Cited by (54)

    • The effect of context on mind-wandering in younger and older adults

      2022, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      According to this hypothesis, the more control an individual engages to defend goal-directed thought in a given situation, the less likely they are to MW. The cognitive resource account can elegantly explain one of the best-established findings in the MW literature, namely that older adults report fewer instances of off-task thought than younger adults (e.g., Giambra, 1989, 2000; Gyurkovics, Balota, & Jackson, 2018; Jackson & Balota, 2012; Jackson, Weinstein, & Balota, 2013; Krawietz, Tamplin, & Radvansky, 2012; Maillet & Schacter, 2016a; Maillet et al., 2018; Seli, Maillet, Smilek, Oakman, & Schacter, 2017; Staub, Doignon-Camus, Bacon, & Bonnefond, 2014; Shake, Shulley, & Soto-Freita, 2015; Zavagnin, Borella, & De Beni et al., 2014). According to this account, the negative relationship between age and MW frequency can be explained as a decrease in available cognitive resources with age (e.g., Craik, Klix, & Hagendorf, 1986; Salthouse, 2009), leading to a reduced ability to maintain off-task thoughts while engaged in a task.

    • Validity of attention self-reports in younger and older adults

      2021, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      We addressed a concern raised by Frank et al. (2015) that younger and older adults might differ in their criterion for classifying attention focus using dichotomous categories such as on-task vs. MW. Previous research has shown that older adults report fewer MW than younger adults and higher proportion of task-related interference thoughts (Frank et al., 2015; McVay et al., 2013; Zavagnin et al., 2014), but the reasons for these differences are yet under debate. Here, we used a continuous rating scale to minimize the possibility that criterion changes between groups in classifying thoughts in one category vs. the other would obscure the assessment of the relationship between self-reports and performance.

    • Differential effects of mind-wandering and visual distraction on age-related changes in neuro-electric brain activity and variability

      2020, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      The age differences in on-task and mind-wandering responses replicate our prior results (Maillet et al., in press), whereas the finding of more thoughts about environmental distractors and sensations was numerically in the same direction but not significant in this prior study. We note that at least one other study has also reported more thoughts about environmental distractors/sensations in young versus older adults (Zavagnin et al., 2014). This effect may be somewhat counterintuitive given that older adults are more affected by the visual distraction manipulation in the current study.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text