Elsevier

Acta Psychologica

Volume 129, Issue 2, October 2008, Pages 297-307
Acta Psychologica

Sequential effects within a short foreperiod context: Evidence for the conditioning account of temporal preparation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.08.005Get rights and content

Abstract

Responses to an imperative stimulus (IS) are especially fast when they are preceded by a warning signal (WS). When the interval between WS and IS (the foreperiod, FP) is variable, reaction time (RT) is not only influenced by the current FP but also by the FP of the preceding trial. These sequential effects have recently been proposed to originate from a trace conditioning process, in which the individuals learn the temporal WS–IS relationship in a trial-by-trial manner. Research has shown that trace conditioning is maximal when the temporal interval between the conditioned and unconditioned stimulus is between 0.25 and 0.60 s. Consequently, one would predict that sequential effects occur especially within short FP contexts. However, this prediction is contradicted by Karlin [Karlin, L. (1959). Reaction time as a function of foreperiod duration and variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 185–191] who did not observe the typical sequential effects with short FPs. To investigate temporal preparation for short FPs, three experiments were conducted, examining the sequential FP effect comparably for short and long FP-sets (Experiment 1), assessing the influence of catch trials (Experiment 2) and the case of a very dense FP-range (Experiment 3) on sequential FP effects. The results provide strong evidence for sequential effects within a short FP context and thus support the trace conditioning account of temporal preparation.

Introduction

In reaction time (RT) tasks, a warning signal (WS) typically precedes the imperative response stimulus (IS). Since the pioneering work of Woodrow (1914), it has been repeatedly shown that RT is strongly influenced by the interval between the WS and the IS, that is, by the foreperiod (FP, Niemi & Näätänen, 1981, for a review). This FP effect depends on whether the FP duration varies randomly from trial-to-trial (variable FP condition) or remains constant within a block of trials and only varies across blocks (constant FP condition). In the constant condition, mean RT usually increases progressively as the FP duration is increased. In the variable condition, however, mean RT usually decreases as the FP duration increases. These two FP effects are well-established and they can be observed for both simple and choice RT tasks (Bertelson and Boons, 1960, Mattes and Ulrich, 1997, Sanders, 1998). Since the WS conveys no information about the response, these effects reflect a state of non-specific preparation, sometimes referred to as temporal preparation (Müller-Gethmann et al., 2003, Rolke and Hofmann, 2007).

The traditional view of temporal preparation presupposes that participants intentionally prepare for the moment when the IS is delivered (Los & Van den Heuvel, 2001). Central to this view is the assumption that a high preparatory state can be maintained only for a brief duration, that is, 0.1–0.3 s (Alegria, 1974, Gottsdanker, 1975). Accordingly, the individuals need to synchronize this brief preparation period with the moment of IS presentation, because optimal performance can only be achieved when the IS is occurring during this preparation period. However, the individual’s strategy to anticipate the imperative moment, that is, the moment of IS presentation (Los & Van den Heuvel, 2001) greatly differs between the constant FP condition and the variable one. In the constant condition, the individual’s ability to predict the imperative moment deteriorates as FP is lengthened, which in turn impairs the synchronization of the preparation period with the imperative moment at longer FPs (Näätänen, Muranen, & Merisalo, 1974). Accordingly, RT typically increases with increasing FP-length in the constant condition.

In the variable condition, however, there is not only one possible moment but several critical moments at which the IS may occur. For example, if the IS occurs with equal probability at each critical moment, the conditional probability of IS presentation during a single trial increases gradually as time goes by, that is, as the FP ages (Niemi & Näätänen, 1981, p. 137). It is usually believed that individuals become aware of this probability increase. As a result, their expectancy about IS occurrence growths gradually with the aging of FP. This growth of expectancy is assumed to enlarge the preparatory state, producing short RTs at a long FPn and thus accounting for the observed FP–RT effect in the variable condition (Niemi and Näätänen, 1981, Sollers and Hackley, 1997). Thus, the classical view can explain the basic FP–RT effects.

Los and coworkers (Los and Heslenfeld, 2005, Los et al., 2001, Los and Van den Heuvel, 2001), however, have recently challenged this traditional view of an intentionally driven preparation process. They put forward a completely different theoretical viewpoint, arguing that response-related preparation is driven by a process of trace conditioning. In this form of classical conditioning, the unconditioned stimulus (US) is not simultaneously presented together with the conditioned stimulus (CS) but somewhat after the CS. In this situation, the CS can produce response-related activation at the moment when the US will occur (Gallistel and Gibbon, 2000, Grossberg and Merill, 1992, Machado, 1997). Pertaining to the case of temporal preparation, Los and Van den Heuvel (2001) pointed on the conceptual similarity between the trace conditioning paradigm and the temporal preparation paradigm. According to the authors, the IS corresponds to the US, whereas the WS acts as the CS that unintentionally initiates response-related activation at critical moments. In particular, their model relies on four assumptions (cf. Los & Van den Heuvel, 2001, p. 372). First, the conditioned response has scalar property, that is, the preparatory peak is sharp-edged for early critical moments but takes more time to build up and decay when the critical moment is more remote from the WS. Second, the conditioned strength at a critical moment is reinforced when the IS occurs at this moment. Third, the conditioned strength at a critical moment remains unchanged when the IS occurs at an earlier critical moment, and fourth, decreases when the IS occurs at a later critical moment. Los (2004, p. 120) further specified this assumption arguing that when a critical moment is bypassed, it is subject to conditioned inhibition and therefore becomes associated with non-responding. This model refers to RT as a dependent measure, which is inversely related to the strength of the conditioned response at the imperative moment.

In the constant FP condition, activation builds up only at the imperative moment. In the variable FP condition, however, the IS always occurs at random times after the WS; hence reliable response strength cannot develop. In this situation, the individuals have been shown to prepare according to FP-length of the preceding trial (Los & Van den Heuvel, 2001). That is, reinforced response strength from the previous trial carries over to the next trial and elicits response-related activation at the moment which was imperative in the previous trial. Hence, especially short RTs are implied when the FP of the preceding trial is repeated. In fact, this trial-to-trial reinforcement can readily account for the finding that RT decreases with FP in a variable FP condition (see, Los & van den Heuvel, 2001).

As indicated just before, this trial-to-trial reinforcement also implies predictions about intertrial sequential effects that have been repeatedly observed in variable FP experiments. In brief, it has often been reported that, when a particular FP is preceded by a longer one in the preceding trial, RT is longer than when the preceding FP is equally long or shorter (e.g., Baumeister and Joubert, 1969, Karlin, 1959, Schupp and Schlier, 1972, Vallesi et al., 2007, Van der Lubbe et al., 2004, Woodrow, 1914, Zahn and Rosenthal, 1966). These asymmetrical sequential FP effects have become the principal argument for demonstrating the superiority of the conditioning view over the classical view. Whereas the classical view cannot suitably account for sequential effects, the conditioning view provides a rather direct and plausible account (Los & Van den Heuvel, 2001, p. 371).

There are three possible FP sequences in the variable FP condition. First, a FP can be repeated in the subsequent trial. As mentioned before, RT is predicted to be short on the subsequent trial, because response strength was reinforced at the imperative moment in the preceding trial. Second, the FP can alter from long to short. In this case, a long RT should result because the imperative moment was not reinforced in the preceding trial. Finally, the FP can alter from short to long. In this case, the conditioning account predicts relatively short RTs, because later imperative moments are less frequently bypassed and thus less frequently associated with non-responding. Accordingly, response strength to an IS should increase with FP-length and should be maximal at the latest imperative moment (see Los, 2004, p. 120, for a detailed explanation). Hence, the conditioning view implies an asymmetric sequential FP effect in that a long FPn−1 prolongs RT in a subsequent trial with a short FPn, whereas a short FPn−1 should not produce such a prolongation.

Most studies that have reported this asymmetrical sequential effect employed FPs with a mean FP usually above one second (Appendix 1). The choice of these FP-sets appears somewhat suboptimal, since substantial empirical evidence has shown that human trace conditioning in conventional settings is usually maximal for CS–US intervals between 0.25 and 0.60 s (see Anderson, 2000, Mauk and Buonomano, 2004). This notion also agrees with the predictions of formal conditioning models (e.g., Machado, 1997, Moore et al., 1998, pp. 4–8; Sutton & Barto, 1998, chap. 6). Specifically, the core assumption of these models is that a CS initiates a cascade of neural activation and when the US occurs during this process, an associative link is established between the representation of the CS and the one of the US, that is, these two representations become “time-tagged” (Moore et al., 1998, Osman et al., 2006). The neural activation triggered by the CS, however, decays within a few seconds and, consequently, the CS–US linkage is particularly effective at short intervals but less effective at long ones. Hence, according to trace conditioning models, one should also expect an asymmetrical sequential FP effects in a short variable FP-set.

However, unlike conventional settings of trace conditioning (e.g., human eyelid conditioning) this prediction is not confirmed within the context of mental chronometry, in which mean RT typically serves as measure of performance. Karlin (1959) examined sequential FP effects with a very short FP-set. In one condition, FPs were 1.6, 2.0, and 2.4 s and the typical asymmetrical sequential FP effect was observed; in another condition, the FPs were especially short, that is, 0.4, 0.5, and 0.6 s. In this condition, an anomalous sequential FP effect was obtained, which differed entirely from those obtained at longer FPs. Specifically, RT increased with increasing FPn after the presentation of a short FPn−1, instead of the typical decrease. Furthermore, the mean FP–RT function in this condition actually increased rather than decreased with FP-length. Hence, Karlin’s study provides conflicting data for the conditioning view. If sequential FP effects are the signature of trace conditioning, as proposed by Los and Van den Heuvel (2001), one would expect a clear asymmetrical sequential FP effect within this short variable FP context.

There are several factors that might be responsible for the abnormal RT pattern in Karlin’s (1959) study. First, one may argue that immediate arousal effects elicited by the WS are operating at this short FP-set and thus override the effects of temporal preparation (Bertelson & Tisseyre, 1969). However, this explanation seems unlikely since arousal is largely dependent on WS intensity (Ulrich & Mattes, 1996), and this intensity was low (30 dB) in Karlin’s study. Second, Karlin employed a simple instead of a choice RT task. It is therefore possible that premature responses (no catch trials were used) or occasional responses to the WS (the same tone functioned as WS and as IS) concealed the sequential FP effect. Third, Karlin used a very dense FP-range (FPs: 0.4, 0.5, 0.6 s) which may not have induced sufficient temporal uncertainty to reveal a sequential FP modulation on RT (see, Klemmer, 1957, Niemi and Näätänen, 1981). Instead of adapting temporal preparation from trial-to-trial, the individuals may have always prepared for the shortest imperative moment, resulting in optimal performance in short FPn trials but suboptimal performance in medium or long FPn trials.

Unlike Karlin, 1959, Alegria, 1975b found a flattened standard FP-RT effect using a very dense FP-range (0.6, 0.7, 0.8 s). Since Alegria (1975b) used a choice RT task and FPs above 0.6 s, it is not clear why his results differed from Karlin’s (1959) observation. Hence, the question of how individuals prepare for the IS at very short variable FPs remains unclear. Moreover, the divergence between findings in conventional trace conditioning research (Mauk and Buonomano, 2004, Moore et al., 1998) and in the context of chronometric RT research (Alegria, 1975b, Karlin, 1959) clearly shows that this question is not trivial.

In the present study, three experiments were conducted to study the cognitive processes underlying temporal preparation within a very short temporal context. In order to address our major question, whether there is evidence for an asymmetrical sequential FP effect within a short variable FP context, Experiments 1 and 2 examined temporal preparation with FPs below 0.6 s using an auditory WS and a visual IS. To ensure that temporal uncertainty imposed by the FP variability (Klemmer, 1957, p. 198) is sufficient, the FP-range was larger than in Karlin’s (1959) short FP-set. Experiment 1 used stimuli similar to the ones used by Los et al. (2001), a condition with three short FPs (0.2, 0.4, 0.6 s), and a control condition with three longer FPs (1.2, 2.4, 3.6 s). A choice RT task was employed to control for anticipatory responses. Experiment 2 examined sequential effects within a short FP context only (0.2, 0.4, 0.6 s), using a simple RT task. To investigate the role of anticipatory responses for the sequential FP effect, a condition with no catch trials was compared to a condition with 25% catch trials. Finally, a supplementary goal was to clarify the reasons why Karlin did observe an abnormal RT pattern in his study. Experiment 3 therefore more directly replicated Karlin’s experiment, using a very dense FP-range (FPs: 0.4, 0.5, 0.6 s). In addition to Karlin’s simple RT task, we also employed a choice RT task.

Section snippets

Experiment 1

In Experiment 1, we used two FP-sets to assess whether there is evidence for sequential effects within a short temporal context, and whether short FPs reveal a similar asymmetrical pattern of sequential FP effects compared to long FPs. Anticipatory responding was controlled by using a choice RT task. In the short FP condition, we employed three FPs of 0.2, 0.4, and 0.6 s, whereas in the long FP condition, three FPs of 1.2, 2.4, and 3.6 s were used. These FP-durations were selected to keep the

Experiment 2

In Experiment 2 we assessed sequential FP effects in a simple RT task employing only the short FP-set of Experiment 1 (FPs: 0.2, 0.4, 0.6 s). By means of a simple RT task, we aimed to examine the role of anticipatory responses on the asymmetrical sequential FP effect. In particular, we employed the catch trial technique to control for anticipatory responses; that is, we compared the asymmetrical sequential FP effect in a condition with 0% catch trials (referred to as no-CT condition) to a

Experiment 3

The results of the Experiments 1 and 2 provide clear-cut evidence for an asymmetrical sequential FP effect within an FP context below 0.6 s, and thus confirmed the predictions derived from the trace conditioning account. However, since Experiment 2 demonstrated that anticipatory responding does not alter the asymmetrical pattern of the sequential FP effect, it still remains unclear why Karlin (1959) did not observe the typical RT pattern in a condition with short FPs. An alternative yet

General discussion

In the present study, we examined sequential effects in variable FP experiments, which have recently been proposed to originate from a trace conditioning process (Los & Van den Heuvel, 2001). Since trace conditioning in conventional settings is most effective when the FP interval is especially short (i.e., between 0.25 and 0.60 s), a clear-cut asymmetrical sequential FP effect should be observed within a short FP context. In contrast to this prediction, Karlin (1959) did not find the typical

Conclusion

In line with the predictions derived from conventional trace conditioning research, the present study demonstrates evidence that temporal trial-to-trial adaptation occurs within a very short variable FP context. This was independent of whether a choice RT or a simple RT task was used, and independent of whether anticipatory responses were prevented by employing the catch trial technique (Experiments 1 and 2). However, if the FP-range is dense and does not provide sufficient temporal

Acknowledgements

Parts of this study were presented at the 49th Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (TeaP), Trier, March 26–28, 2007, and at the Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Edinburgh, Scotland, July 4–7, 2007. We thank Sander Los and Jeff Miller for very helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper. Further we thank Karin Bausenhart, Allen Osman, Robert Langner and Hannes Schröter for many discussions on particular issues. Financial support for this research was provided by the

References (59)

  • R.H.J. Van der Lubbe et al.

    Being prepared on time: on the importance of the previous foreperiod to current preparation, as reflected in speed, force and preparation-related brain potentials

    Acta Psychologica

    (2004)
  • J. Alegria

    The time course of preparation after a first peak: Some constraints of reacting mechanisms

    Quarterly Journal Experimental Psychology

    (1974)
  • J. Alegria

    Sequential effects of foreperiod duration as a function of the frequency of foreperiod repetitions

    Journal of Motor Behavior

    (1975)
  • J. Alegria

    Sequential effects of foreperiod duration: Some strategical factors in tasks involving time uncertainty

  • J.R. Anderson

    Learning and memory: An integrated approach

    (2000)
  • A.A. Baumeister et al.

    Interactive effects on reaction time of preparatory interval length and preparatory interval frequency

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1969)
  • K.M. Bausenhart et al.

    The locus of temporal preparation effects: Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm

    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

    (2006)
  • P. Bertelson et al.

    Time uncertainty and choice reaction time

    Nature

    (1960)
  • D.H. Brainard

    The psychophysics toolbox

    Spatial Vision

    (1997)
  • C.H. Brunia

    Waiting in readiness: Gating in attention and motor preparation

    Psychophysiology

    (1993)
  • A. Correa et al.

    Endogenous temporal orienting of attention in detection and discrimination tasks

    Perception & Psychophysics

    (2004)
  • D.H. Drazin

    Effects of foreperiod, foreperiod variability, and probability of stimulus occurrence on simple reaction time

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1961)
  • C.R. Gallistel et al.

    Time, rate, and conditioning

    Psychological Review

    (2000)
  • R. Gottsdanker

    The attaining and maintaining of preparation

  • M. Granjon et al.

    Influence of an intertrial interval on sequential effects related to preparatory period duration for reaction time-task

    Perceptual and Motor Skills

    (1979)
  • M. Granjon et al.

    Effect of length of runs of repetitions on the RT–ISI relationship

    Quarterly Journal Experimental Psychology

    (1977)
  • M. Grosjean et al.

    Timing and reaction time

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (2001)
  • L. Karlin

    Reaction time as a function of foreperiod duration and variability

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1959)
  • E.T. Klemmer

    Time uncertainty in simple reaction time

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1956)
  • Cited by (88)

    • Warning signals only support the first action in a sequence

      2023, Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text