Elsevier

Acta Psychologica

Volume 125, Issue 2, June 2007, Pages 144-154
Acta Psychologica

Memories affect mood: Evidence from covert experimental assignment to positive, neutral, and negative memory recall

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.07.009Get rights and content

Abstract

Memory recall has been proposed as a common and effective mood regulation strategy. Although several studies have presented results suggesting that recalling valenced memories affects subsequent mood, their designs allow for alternative interpretations of the observed effects. Two such alternatives include the reverse effect (mood effects on memory due to non-experimental assignment to memory recall condition) and demand characteristics of the experiment. We used covert experimental assignment to memory condition, asking subjects (N = 314; 56% female) to recall memories that were primarily positive, neutral, or negative. Results showed the expected effect on mood (p < .002), with reported mood worst in the negative memory condition, better in the neutral condition, and best in the positive condition. These results suggest that valenced memory recall does indeed exert an effect on mood, and may do so even without the individual’s awareness.

Introduction

One of the fundamental assumptions about the relation between thought and feeling is that memories and mood affect one another. Generally the effects are assumed to be reciprocal: An individual’s mood influences the valence of memories that the person recalls (see Bower, 1981, Eich and Metcalfe, 1989), and thinking about memories with a certain valence can affect a person’s mood. Numerous studies have demonstrated the former effect—that a person’s mood can influence the types of memories that the person recalls (e.g., Parrott and Sabini, 1990, Singer and Salovey, 1988).

The question of the reverse effect—that the memories a person chooses to think about can affect his or her subsequent mood—is an important one. Memory recall has been proposed as a common and effective (conscious or unconscious) mood-regulation strategy (e.g., Forgas, 2000, Joormann and Siemer, 2004, Josephson et al., 1996, McFarland and Buehler, 1997, Rusting and DeHart, 2000). One of the advantages of using memories to regulate one’s mood is that this strategy can be used in virtually any setting, which is not the case with many mood regulation behaviors that individuals commonly report using (e.g., going shopping, eating something, having sex; Thayer, Newman, & McClain, 1994). It is important, therefore, to determine whether a person can influence his or her mood by recalling specifically valenced memories.

The hypothesized effect of memory on mood is intuitively plausible; most everyone has experienced an improvement in mood after recalling a happy memory, or has felt sadness when recalling a memory involving hurt or loss. However, other relationships between memory and mood are possible. One possibility is simply the null hypothesis—that the valence of recalled memories has no effect on subsequent mood. Under this scenario, perceived effects of memories on mood might be nothing more than “hand waving”, with motivated attempts to improve mood being the real driver of the mood improvement, while the happy memories are simply epiphenomenal. Another possibility that is intuitively plausible is that memories actually affect moods in an incongruent direction, due to a comparison effect. For example, an individual who remembers the most recent time he or she had the flu might feel relieved that now he or she is well, and would experience an improvement in mood. Similar effects are plausible given the recall of happier times (e.g., the person’s wedding day), which might by comparison lead to a decrement in mood, since the person perceives him- or herself to be in a worse situation at present (e.g., experiencing marital conflict).

Distinguishing between these various hypotheses requires demonstrating that valenced memory recall causally influences moods, which is not an easy task. Simply ordering the collection of data in a logical way—by asking participants to report a memory and then having them report their mood—is not sufficient. If there is a correlation between memory valence and reported mood, it could be the case that the mood was primary and was responsible for the valence of the reported memory. This problem requires that participants be randomly assigned to recall positively- or negatively-valenced memories. In this way, prior mood cannot account for an observed association between the valence of recalled memory and the participant’s subsequent mood.

However, random assignment alone is not sufficient. If participants are aware that an experimental manipulation is intended to change their mood, it is impossible to rule out the effect of demand characteristics on reported mood (see, e.g., Buchwald et al., 1981, Polivy and Doyle, 1980). If participants are told, for example, to “think of a very happy memory”, it is likely that their subsequently reported mood will be contaminated with their perception that the experimenter is expecting them to report a positive mood. Similarly, the use of explicit mood induction procedures in many studies prior to asking participants to recall potentially mood-altering memories (e.g., Erber and Erber, 1994, Joormann and Siemer, 2004, Rusting and DeHart, 2000) can alert the participants that their moods are “supposed” to change in response to the experimental stimuli (see a meta-analysis by Westermann, Kordelia, Stahl, & Hesse, 1996, for a discussion of the influence of demand characteristics on the effectiveness of mood induction procedures). Such demand characteristics could lead to an erroneous rejection of the null hypothesis that recalled memory valence does not affect mood. Therefore, in addition to random assignment to memory condition, participants should not be made aware that the memories they are recalling have a specific, mood-relevant valence.

A large body of research is based on the assumption that recalling positively valenced memories is a strategy for regulating negative moods. For example, Forgas’s (2000) dual-process theory of mood regulation holds that individuals in negative moods are motivated to recall mood-incongruent information in order to maintain affective homeostasis; Boden and Baumeister (1997) suggested that the tendency by some individuals to recall more positive memories after watching an unpleasant film was an effort to regulate their moods; and Parrott and Sabini (1990) interpreted mood-incongruent memory recall as an attempt to regulate mood (see also Forgas and Ciarrochi, 2002, Joormann and Siemer, 2004, McFarland and Buehler, 1997, Parkinson and Totterdell, 1999, Thayer et al., 1994). Although many studies have reported effects of recalled memories on mood (e.g., Josephson et al., 1996, Rusting and DeHart, 2000, Setliff and Marmurek, 2002), their designs do not permit strong conclusions to be drawn about the direction of the effect. These design features represent not an insignificant methodological nuance, but rather a significant gap in the mood and memory literature.

The present study provides a more definitive answer to the question of whether recalling memories can influence individuals’ moods because to our knowledge it is the first study to satisfy both of the conditions outlined above: random assignment to memory valence, with participants blind to their assignment. These conditions were accomplished by asking participants to report memories that, unbeknownst to the participants, were either primarily negative, neutral, or positive, depending on which version of the questionnaire the participant received, followed by mood ratings that were supposedly unrelated to the memory questions. As such, this study addresses the unresolved issue of whether memories influence mood or whether apparent memory effects on mood are actually mood effects on memory, or result from demand characteristics of the experiments.

Section snippets

Participants and design overview

Participants were adults waiting for trains at the main train station in a large city in the eastern United States. Experimenters approached every individual who appeared to be over 18 years of age and who was not obviously busy (e.g., talking on a cell phone, tending to small children). The experimenters explained that they were conducting a research study and asked whether the individual would be willing to take “3 or 4 min to fill out an anonymous questionnaire for the study”. If the

Awareness of study hypothesis

A small number of participants (12/326; 3.6%) correctly guessed the hypothesis of the present study (e.g., “Something about answering these questions … will affect my mood”); typical incorrect guesses missed the mark widely, given the between subjects design and the apparent randomness of the questions, and included responses such as “some type of survey of American culture”, “how many people will agree to fill out a survey in a train station”, “matching ethnicity with habits”, and “short term

Discussion

As predicted, individuals who were induced to recall more positive memories reported better mood than those induced to recall neutral or negative memories. The design of the current study allowed us to distinguish between the mood-altering effects of memory recall per se and other factors such as motivational components that are involved in more explicit efforts to use memories to influence one’s mood.

These results have several important implications. First, they suggest that thinking of

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Sara R. Jaffee, Ph.D., for helpful comments on a draft of this article. We appreciate the cheerful participation of so many research participants at the train station. The writing of this article was supported by NIH F31 MH-073363-01A1 to SJG and NIH R01 AG-14082-04, NIH K02 AG-00756-04, NIH R01 DA14129, and NSF 0226060 to MJF.

References (23)

  • M. Martin

    On the induction of mood

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (1990)
  • A.E. Setliff et al.

    The mood regulatory function of autobiographical recall is moderated by self-esteem

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2002)
  • J.A. Singer et al.

    Mood and memory: Evaluating the network theory of affect

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (1988)
  • J. Boden et al.

    Repressive coping: distraction using pleasant thoughts and memories

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1997)
  • G. Bower

    Mood and memory

    American Psychologist

    (1981)
  • D. Brewer et al.

    Induction of mood and mood shift

    Journal of Clinical Psychology

    (1980)
  • A.M. Buchwald et al.

    Demand characteristics and the Velten mood induction procedure

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1981)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences

    (1988)
  • E. Eich et al.

    Mood dependent memory for internal versus external events

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    (1989)
  • R. Erber et al.

    Beyond mood and social judgment: mood-incongruent recall and mood regulation

    European Journal of Social Psychology

    (1994)
  • J.P. Forgas

    Managing moods: toward a dual-process theory of spontaneous mood regulation

    Psychological Inquiry

    (2000)
  • Cited by (28)

    • Remembering happy times instead of sticking to negative memories after social exclusion

      2020, Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      Overall, this finding that excluded participants were quicker to learn the positive-reward association could be interpreted as a type of mood repair strategy, where social exclusion leads to enhanced striving for positive memories, as a defense against the rejection-prompted negative mood and lowered self-esteem. Recalling positive memories does lead to an improved mood (Gillihan, Kessler, & Farah, 2007) and is a common strategy to repair negative moods (Josephson, Singer, & Salovey, 1996; Rusting & DeHart, 2000). Effective strategies to bolster threatened needs after social exclusion include positive refocusing, a cognitive emotion regulation strategy that involves bringing one's attention to a positive distraction, and self-affirmation, recalling important values and positive characteristics of the self (Timeo, Riva, & Paladino, 2019).

    • Need satisfaction in episodic memories impacts mood at retrieval and well-being over time

      2017, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Indeed, past research has shown that positive memories can be recalled, deliberately or automatically, as a way to repair negative mood (Josephson et al., 1996; Joormann, Siemer, and Gotlib, 2007; Parrott and Sabini, 1990). Gillihan, Kessler, and Farah (2007) showed that participants (n = 326) who were randomly assigned to recall positive episodic memories reported a significantly greater mood after memory recollection than participants who were randomly assigned to recall negative memories. In another study (Baker and Guttfreund, 1993), 80 community college students rated their negative mood, were randomly assigned to think of two positive or two negative memories, and finally rated their negative mood again.

    • Memory and place attachment as predictors of imagined restorative perceptions of favourite places

      2016, Journal of Environmental Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      In imposing the restriction that favourite places should be within Finland we sought to maintain ecological validity and to approximate real-life experience. Similarly, our restriction that the recalled place memory should be positive was based on existing literature relating positive memories to positive, and therefore potentially restorative, affective outcomes (cf. Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2003; Gillihan et al., 2007). However, future research without such parameters may afford the opportunity to study potentially negatively valenced aspects of place memory and place attachment, and possible effects on restorative perceptions.

    • Autobiographical memory and well-being in aging: The central role of semantic self-images

      2015, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      It is thus possible that other measures of the content and affective properties of episodic memories, such as redemption and contamination themes (McAdams, Reynolds, Lewis, Patten, & Bowman, 2001) or intrinsic versus integrated themes (Bauer et al., 2005), would yield significant relationships with well-being. Crucially, few studies that measure the relationship between episodic memory and mood or well-being (e.g., Gillihan, Kessler, & Farah, 2007; Philippe et al., 2012) also include measures of semantic autobiographical memory, such as self-images. Thus it is possible that when memories have been shown to have an impact on well-being, they may have done so via their relationship with semantic facts about the self.

    • The interplay between memory control and emotion regulation

      2024, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text