Elsevier

Acta Psychologica

Volume 115, Issue 1, January 2004, Pages 35-42
Acta Psychologica

Mental effort causes vigilance decrease due to resource depletion

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.11.001Get rights and content

Abstract

The resource view on vigilance performance was tested. First, a low demanding task was compared with a similar low demanding task in which stimulus presentation was less monotonous due to added, irrelevant, stimuli. The resource view, maintaining that vigilance is lowered by hard mental work, predicts that addition of irrelevant stimuli will not affect performance. The classic arousal theory, however, states that arousal drops due to monotonous stimulus presentation and predicts that decreasing monotony will enhance performance. Results showed that performance was unaffected by added stimuli. Second, we tested whether a high-demanding task (with identical stimulus presentation as the low demanding task, but different instruction) would cause a greater decline in performance than the low demanding task. Indeed, in the high-demanding task performance was affected most. In sum, it appears that vigilance decreases due to hard mental work, which requires many resources. Both overall performance and decrement in performance can be explained in terms of resources, and this suggests that vigilance tasks should be resource-demanding tasks, which do not have to be of long duration.

Introduction

Performance on sustained attention tasks is known to decrease with time on task. One of the first theories that sought to explain this decrement was the arousal theory, a derivative of arousal theories on general human performance (see e.g. Eysenck, 1982). This approach argues that the decrement in performance is due to lack of stimulation, needed to maintain alertness at a required level (Ballard, 1996; Eysenck, 1982). More specifically, it assumes that a state of vigilance cannot be maintained under conditions of repetitive stimulation (Parasuraman, 1985), due to perceptual habituation (Mackworth, 1969; Stroh, 1971). Mackworth maintained that monotonous stimulus presentation causes habituation. This, in turn, decreases arousal and thus results in a performance decrement (Mackworth, 1969). The arousal theory predicts that a greater complexity of the stimulus situation results in a smaller decrement, which has indeed been found (see Stroh, 1971, for an overview).

Some findings do not corroborate with the arousal theory’s predictions. First, arousal primarily affects overall performance, rather than performance decrement (Jewett, Dijk, Kronauer, & Dinges, 1999; Parasuraman, 1998). However, decrement effects have been found in some studies (Smit, Eling, & Coenen, 2002). Second, participants report a substantial processing load during vigilance tasks (Hoffman, Sherrick, & Warm, 1998). This suggests that vigilance tasks are not merely boring, but can be mentally demanding.

Another explanation for vigilance decrements may be found in the resource or capacity view on performance. Resource models have been considered in the literature on general performance (Parasuraman, 1998; Wickens & Kessel, 1980), but can also be applied to sustained attention or vigilance research (see Matthews & Davies, 2001; Warm & Dember, 1998). The resource view asserts that energy or resources are needed during task performance and these resources are limited in their availability (Wickens, 1984). Especially controlled processes, in contrast to automatic processes, require resources (Parasuraman, 1985). Furthermore, as tasks become more difficult, they require more resources (Wickens, 1984). In dual-tasks, often used to test resource models, performance is indeed worse than in single tasks in some studies (Gorissen & Eling, 1998; Matthews, Davies, Westerman, & Stammers, 2000). However, interpretation is hindered by the fact that not only instructions are dissimilar between tasks, but usually also the amount and nature of stimuli. Direct comparison of dual and single tasks is problematic as long as these variables are not held constant.

Prolonged performance on tasks is thought to deplete the pool of resources (Parasuraman, 1985), but it is not exactly clear how this depletion works (Matthews et al., 2000). Unfortunately, cognitively demanding tasks have rarely been used in studies on sustained attention performance (Matthews & Davies, 2001). Task difficulty in this field of research is generally manipulated by decreasing discriminability of stimuli (Parasuraman & Mouloua, 1987). Cognitively demanding tasks have been used in the form of dual-tasks, but, as stated above, comparison with single tasks has been problematic due to differences between single and dual-tasks with respect to the nature and amount of presented stimuli. This experiment was conducted with the aim of directly testing resource view predictions with respect to long-term performance on a cognitively demanding task, which is directly compared with an equivalent, but low demanding task.

We examined whether additional irrelevant stimuli influence performance. A simple task was compared with a similar simple task that only differed with respect to occasional presentation of irrelevant stimuli. According to the resource view, additional stimuli should either have a small detrimental effect or no effect at all if they are properly ignored. The arousal theory predicts that these additional stimuli, since they decrease monotony in stimulus presentation, should lead to a smaller decrement in performance. We then directly tested the resource prediction that vigilance is declined most in a complex task that requires many processing resources. Stimulus presentation in the complex task was identical to the task with additional stimuli, in order to make direct comparison possible. Merely the instructions changed.

Section snippets

Participants

Seventeen students (three men and 14 women; mean age: 21 years) participated and signed an informed consent. They fulfilled a course requirement or received 20, -for partaking.

Design and procedure

There were four conditions of 50 min each: a control condition (in which participants conversed with the experimenter and read magazines); a simple, low demanding task (sLDT) condition; a composite, low demanding task (cLDT) and a high-demanding task (HDT) condition. Participants were subjected to all conditions.

Each

Statistical analyses

Data were analyzed with repeated measures ANOVAs with Bonferroni’s confidence interval adjustments and Pearson’s bivariate correlations. Task data were analyzed with a 3 × 2 (Condition × Part) ANOVA; there are only three conditions in this ANOVA, as the control condition did not include performance data. Subjective data were computed per dimension of the Thayer scale and analyzed with a 4 × 2 (Condition × Time: before and after task) ANOVA.

Task data

For performance data, see Table 1. For RTs, a main effect of

Discussion

Contrary to the arousal theory prediction, performance remained stable throughout the sLDT and cLDT: no decrement was found. Although it is often stated that vigilance tasks need to last long, decrements have been reported in tasks that have durations of less than 10 min (Matthews, Davies, & Holley, 1993; Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, 1997). A 30-min duration is usually sufficient to yield decrement effects (Matthews et al., 2000). The fact that complexity of the stimulus

Acknowledgements

We thank Sylvia Claassen for assistance during the experiment and Hubert Voogd for programming the software of the mental tasks.

References (28)

  • G. Matthews et al.

    Individual differences in energetic arousal and sustained attention: a dual-task study

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2001)
  • I.H. Robertson et al.

    ‘Oops!’ Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects

    Neuropsychologia

    (1997)
  • J.C. Ballard

    Computerized assessment of sustained attention: a review of factors affecting vigilance performance

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (1996)
  • P.J. Bushnell

    Sustained attention and vigilance: a reply to Sarter and McGaughy

    Psychopharmacology

    (1998)
  • A. Craig

    Signal detection theory and probability matching apply to vigilance

    Human Factors

    (1987)
  • M.W. Eysenck

    Attention and arousal

    (1982)
  • M.E.E. Gorissen et al.

    Dual-task performance after diazepam intake: can resource depletion explain the benzodiazepine-induced amnesia?

    Psychopharmacology

    (1998)
  • H.J. Hannay

    Experimental techniques in human neuropsychology

    (1986)
  • R.R. Hoffman et al.

    Viewing psychology as a whole: the integrative science of William N. Dember

    (1998)
  • D. Howland

    An investigation of the performance of the human monitor

    Wright Air Development Center Technical Note

    (1958)
  • M.E. Jewett et al.

    Dose–response relationship between sleep duration and human psychomotor vigilance and subjective alertness

    Sleep

    (1999)
  • T.M. Lanzetta et al.

    Effects of task type and stimulus heterogeneity on the event rate function in sustained attention

    Human Factors

    (1987)
  • J.F. Mackworth

    Vigilance and habituation: a neuropsychological approach

    (1969)
  • G. Matthews

    Signal probability effects on high-workload vigilance tasks

    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

    (1996)
  • Cited by (129)

    • An introduction to air traffic control and the application of human factors

      2022, Human Factors in Aviation and Aerospace, Third Edition
    • Exploration of the effects of task-related fatigue on eye-motion features and its value in improving driver fatigue-related technology

      2021, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour
      Citation Excerpt :

      It is important to understand if the fatigue status is companied by vigilance decrement. Theoretically, many recent analyses (Grier et al., 2003; Smit et al., 2004) strongly suggested that the vigilance decrement is caused by the effort demands of maintaining active attention, rather than the lack of engagement usually assumed. Also, as we discussed in our previous work (Hu & Lodewijks, 2020), an active driving scenario follows the resource depletion theory.

    • The effect of sleep-need and time-on-task on driver fatigue

      2020, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour
      Citation Excerpt :

      A number of mechanisms have been proposed to explain task-related fatigue effects. They include reduced arousal (Dickman, 2002; Duffy, 1957), cognitive resource depletion (Smit, Eling, & Coenen, 2004), impaired action monitoring (Boksem, Meijman, & Lorist, 2006), reduced task engagement or its proposed components (consisting of energetic arousal, motivation and concentration; Atchley, Chan, & Gregersen, 2014; Neubauer, Matthews, & Saxby, 2014), the withdrawal or limitations of attentional effort (Grier, Warm, Dember, Matthews, & Galinsky, 2003; Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, 1997), the strategic control of effort allocation (Hockey, 1997; Hockey, Wastell, & Sauer, 1998), and the build-up of local use-dependent sleep pressure in neuronal groups subserving the task (Van Dongen, Belenky, & Krueger, 2011). There is strong evidence that fatigue outcomes can arise from each source individually (e.g., Atchley & Chan, 2011; Atchley et al., 2014; Hallvig, Anund, Fors, Kecklund, & Akerstedt, 2014; Ingre, Akerstedt, Peters, Anund, & Kecklund, 2006; Lamond & Dawson, 1999; Lenné, Triggs, & Redman, 1997; Ma, Gu, Jia, Yao, & Chang, 2018; Risser & Ware, 1999; Sandberg et al., 2011; Ting et al., 2008; Williamson & Feyer, 2000).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text