Neurobiologic responses to speech in noise in children with learning problems: deficits and strategies for improvement

https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00465-5Get rights and content

Abstract

Objectives: Some children with learning problems (LP) experience speech-sound perception deficits that worsen in background noise. The first goal was to determine whether these impairments are associated with abnormal neurophysiologic representation of speech features in noise reflected at brain-stem and cortical levels. The second goal was to examine the perceptual and neurophysiological benefits provided to an impaired system by acoustic cue enhancements.

Methods: Behavioral speech perception measures (just noticeable difference scores), auditory brain-stem responses, frequency-following responses and cortical-evoked potentials (P1, N1, P1′, N1′) were studied in a group of LP children and compared to responses in normal children.

Results: We report abnormalities in the fundamental sensory representation of sound at brain-stem and cortical levels in the LP children when speech sounds were presented in noise, but not in quiet. Specifically, the neurophysiologic responses from these LP children displayed a different spectral pattern and lacked precision in the neural representation of key stimulus features. Cue enhancement benefited both behavioral and neurophysiological responses.

Conclusions: Overall, these findings contribute to our understanding of the preconscious biological processes underlying perception deficits and may assist in the design of effective intervention strategies.

Introduction

Recent studies indicate that speech-sound perception deficits may contribute to the learning problems (LP) of some children. In particular, these children have difficulty discriminating between acoustically similar sounds (Tallal and Piercy, 1974, Tallal, 1980, Elliott et al., 1989, Stark and Heinz, 1996, Kraus et al., 1996, Bradlow et al., 1999). Moreover, these deficits become worse in the presence of background noise (Nabelek and Pickett, 1974, Elliott, 1979, Bellis, 1996, Chermak and Musiek, 1997). While the underlying cause of speech-sound perception deficits remains controversial (Tallal and Piercy, 1974, Tallal, 1980, Nittrouer, 1992, Studdert-Kennedy and Mody, 1995, Denneberg, 1999), new evidence suggests that basic neurophysiologic processes related to stimulus encoding and discrimination may be involved. Three recent studies have begun to elucidate the biological bases of impaired speech perception in some individuals with LP. First, poor readers differed from good readers in neural recovery time of auditory cortical responses to rapidly presented stimuli (Nagarajan et al., 1999). Second, dyslexic individuals displayed significantly smaller far-field EEG amplitude modulated following responses than normal subjects (McAnally and Stein, 1997). Finally, a subset of children with LP showed a significant reduction in cortical responses to speech-sound contrasts differing in rapid spectro-temporal elements, consistent with their impaired behavioral discrimination of those stimuli (Kraus et al., 1996).

Despite general acknowledgement that background noise excessively taxes perception in most children with LP, little is known about the underlying neurobiologic processes. The first goal of this investigation was to determine whether speech perception deficits in some LP children are associated with abnormal neurophysiologic representation of rapidly changing speech features in noise reflected by potentials generated at brain-stem and cortical levels. To accomplish this aim, auditory brain-stem responses (ABR), frequency-following responses (FFR) and cortical-evoked potentials were studied in a group of LP children and compared to responses in normal children.

Evaluation of these electrophysiologic measures separately and in combination provides a unique opportunity to assess the integrity of central auditory stimulus-timing mechanisms at various levels of the auditory pathway. For instance, the ABR reflects neural activity synchronized to the stimulus onset. It is generated by action potentials traveling along axons in a pattern of short-duration, biphasic responses. The magnitude of the ABR depends on a high degree of synchronized firing among the neurons, such that deviations of tenths of milliseconds are considered diagnostic of brain-stem pathology (Starr and Don, 1988). If there is excessive neural ‘jitter’, which might occur in an impaired auditory system, the separation of individual neural responses by even a fraction of a millisecond could cause responses to cancel each other out. The FFR also depends on a high degree of neural synchrony. It reflects brain-stem-generated, phase-locked responses to the low frequency components of a stimulus (less than 800 Hz) (Worden and Marsh, 1968). Differences between brain-stem and cortical responses are particularly apparent in the overall spectra of the evoked responses (ABR ∼1 kHz, Boston and Moller, 1985; cortical potentials ∼10 Hz, Moller, 1994). Cortical responses reflect the summation of excitatory post-synaptic potentials originating from multiple generator sites in response to stimulus onset and other acoustic features of the stimulus. These slow dendritic events can be separated by several milliseconds and will still sum constructively. Nevertheless, cortical potentials do depend on stimulus-locked synchronous firing across neural ensembles.

The second goal of this study was to examine the perceptual and neurophysiological benefits provided to an impaired system by acoustic cue enhancements typical of ‘clear’ speech (Picheny et al., 1986) in order to gain a deeper understanding of how processing deficits can be overcome by the speech signal. Research has shown that speakers naturally alter the acoustic characteristics of their speech from a ‘conversational’ to a ‘clear’ speaking style when the listener is known to have speech perception difficulties. The acoustic characteristics of ‘conversational’ and ‘clear’ speech have been well described (Picheny et al., 1986). The perceptual benefits of ‘clear’ speech have also been established (Picheny et al., 1985, Gordon-Salant, 1986, Hazan and Simpson, 1998), and some of these features have been incorporated into commercially available auditory training programs designed for LP children (Merzenich et al., 1996, Tallal et al., 1996).

Section snippets

Subjects

Subjects consisted of normal children (n=9) and children with LP (n=9). The normal group included children between the ages of 10 and 13 years (3 female, 6 male) with no reported history of learning or attention problems and scores within normal limits on standardized tests of learning and academic achievement (Woodcock and Johnson, 1977, Woodcock and Johnson, 1989, Wilkinson, 1993). The group with LP included age-matched children (two female, 7 male) diagnosed clinically with a reading-based

Behavioral perception

In the behavioral experiment, JNDs were obtained to the ‘conversational’ speech continuum in quiet and in noise and to the ‘clear’ speech continuum in noise for normal and LP children. As expected, a comparison between ‘conversational’ speech in quiet and in noise scores indicated that both groups showed more difficulty discriminating speech in noise (Wilcoxon signed ranks test: normal, T+=45, P=0.008; LP, T+=45, P=0.008). That is, a comparison of ‘conversational’ speech in noise scores between

Discussion

Taken together, the electrophysiologic and behavioral results demonstrate a difference between these normal and LP children in the neurophysiologic representation and perception of speech in noise. Specifically, group differences arose in the JNDs of ‘conversational’ speech in noise, the magnitude of the spectral content in the FFR, the strength of the stimulus-to-response correlation coefficients reflected in the brain-stem response, the latency of wave V in the ABR and the amplitude of

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge T. McGee, D. Koch, C. King, the children, and their families for their valuable contributions. This work was supported by NIH-NIDCD-DC01510.

References (70)

  • H Sohmer et al.

    Sources of frequency following responses (FFR) in man

    Electroenceph clin Neurophysiol Suppl

    (1977)
  • R.D Stillman et al.

    Components of the frequency-following potential in man

    Electroenceph clin Neurophysiol

    (1978)
  • P Tallal et al.

    Developmental aphasia: rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception

    Neuropsychologia

    (1974)
  • M.J Wilson et al.

    Human short-latency auditory responses obtained by cross-correlation

    Electroenceph clin Neurophysiol

    (1987)
  • F.G Worden et al.

    Frequency-following (microphonic-like) neural responses evoked by sound

    Electroenceph clin Neurophysiol

    (1968)
  • M Barinaga

    Listening in on the brain (news)

    Science

    (1998)
  • T.J Bellis

    Assessment and management of central auditory processing disorders in the educational setting: from science to practice

    (1996)
  • S.C Bledsoe et al.

    Response characteristics of cochlear nucleus neurons to 500 Hz tones and noise: findings relating to frequency-following potentials

    J Neurophysiol

    (1982)
  • J Boston et al.

    Brainstem auditory-evoked potentials

    CRC Crit Rev Biomed Eng

    (1985)
  • A.R Bradlow et al.

    Effects of lengthened formant transition duration on discrimination and neural representation of synthetic CV syllables by normal and learning-disabled children

    J Acoust Soc Am

    (1999)
  • R Burkhard et al.

    The effect of broadband noise on the human brainstem auditory evoked response. I. Rate and intensity effects

    J Acoust Soc Am

    (1983)
  • T.D Carrell et al.

    Interactive software for evaluating auditory discrimination

    Ear Hear

    (1999)
  • G.D Chermak et al.

    Central auditory processing disorders: new perspectives

    (1997)
  • J Cunningham et al.

    Speech-evoked neurophysiologic responses in children with learning problems: development and behavioral correlates of perception

    Ear Hear

    (2000)
  • J Cunningham et al.

    Neurophysiologic representation of clear speech in noise

    Assoc Res Otolarygol

    (2001)
  • B Delgutte et al.

    Speech coding in the auditory nerve. V. Vowels in background noise

    J Acoust Soc Am

    (1984)
  • V.H Denneberg

    Speech perception deficits in poor readers: auditory processing or phonological coding?

    J Learn Disabil

    (1999)
  • R.A Dobie et al.

    Short-latency auditory responses obtained by cross correlation

    J Acoust Soc Am

    (1984)
  • J.K Douglass et al.

    Noise enhancement of information transfer in crayfish mechanoreceptors by stochastic resonance

    Nature

    (1993)
  • J.J Eggermont

    The correlative brain: theory and experiment in neural interaction. Studies of brain function

    (1990)
  • L.L Elliott

    Performance of children aged 9 to 17 on a test of speech intelligibility in noise using sentence material with controlled word predictability

    J Acoust Soc Am

    (1979)
  • L.L Elliott et al.

    Fine-grained auditory discrimination in normal children and children with language-learning problems

    J Speech Hear Res

    (1989)
  • G.C Galbraith et al.

    Intelligible speech encoded in the human brain stem frequency-following response

    NeuroReport

    (1995)
  • G.C Galbraith et al.

    Brain stem frequency-following response to dichotic vowels during attention

    NeuroReport

    (1998)
  • S Gordon-Salant

    Recognition of natural and time/intensity altered CVs by young and elderly subjects with normal hearing

    J Acoust Soc Am

    (1986)
  • Cited by (229)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text