Research report
Attention and successful episodic encoding: an event-related potential study

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00066-5Get rights and content

Abstract

Event-related potentials (ERPs) were used to delineate the cerebral processes occurring when information is encoded into episodic memory and to determine how these processes are affected by divided attention. ERPs were recorded during encoding under focused or divided attention, and were selectively averaged on the basis of their retrieval during later free recall and recognition tests (with remember–know judgments). Items retrieved with conscious recollection of the encoding episode (remembered, recalled) were distinguished at encoding from later missed items by an enhanced left fronto-temporal negative wave (N340), a negative posterior sustained potential and a positive frontal sustained potential. These effects occurred independently of the level of attention. Items later retrieved on the basis of familiarity (known) elicited a larger N340 than missed items, but did not demonstrate the increased sustained potentials. We suggest that item-specific conceptual processing (N340) is sufficient to produce familiarity-based recognition, but additional elaborative processing (sustained interaction of frontal and posterior regions) is necessary for conscious recollection. The effect of divided attention on these processes was related to the difficulty of the secondary task, with the more difficult task causing greater and earlier interference.

Introduction

Attention is essential to the formation of a declarative memory trace. Reducing the attentional resources available for encoding information by distraction from a secondary task impairs performance on explicit tests of recall and recognition (e.g. Ref. [9]) but not on implicit tests such as word-fragment completion (e.g. Ref. [65]). In addition, when explicitly recognized items are categorized according to whether the retrieval experience is one of consciously ‘remembering’ the details of an encoding episode, or simply ‘knowing’ that it previously occurred [89], a secondary task at encoding specifically decreases the proportion of ‘remembered’ items (e.g. Ref. [28]). This impairment may occur because the secondary task diverts time and attentional resources away from deep semantic processing. Support for this view comes from studies demonstrating that processing information at a physical or structural level rather than at a deeper, semantic level influences episodic memory performance in a manner similar to divided attention: free recall is impaired [11], and the proportion of items remembered during recognition is reduced [25], [27], [73]. Yet, the stage of processing at which the interference occurs may depend on the degree to which these resources are reduced. Moderate resource reductions may only interfere with relational and elaborative processing [8], whereas more severe reductions may affect basic semantic or even phonemic processing [54].

Moscovitch and Umilta [53] proposed a model of memory and consciousness that describes how reduced resources might interfere with the cerebral processes responsible for episodic memory formation. They propose that the medial temporal lobe/hippocampal complex (MTL/H) only has access to information that has been consciously apprehended. The prefrontal cortex controls the extent to which incoming information is processed into consciousness through the mechanism of selective attention. When attention is divided across multiple tasks, fewer stimuli will reach the level of conscious awareness necessary for apprehension by the MTL/H. Furthermore, those stimuli that do reach this threshold may not be sufficiently encoded because the multitasking required by divided attention tasks occupies the supervisory capacities of the prefrontal cortex and interferes with the ability of this region to engage in the level of elaborative, semantic processing necessary for successful episodic encoding.

Many aspects of this model have been supported by blood flow studies in normal adults. Recent event-related fMRI studies of memory have shown maximal activation of frontal and parahippocampal cortices when items were subsequently recognized with either conscious recollection of the encoding episode (i.e. ‘remembered’) or with high confidence [3], [93]. Items later recognized without either conscious recollection (i.e. ‘known’) or with low confidence did not differ from missed items. Furthermore, PET studies examining the effects of divided attention on episodic encoding have found that reduced resources affect activity in similar regions. Using a visuomotor secondary task and an auditory memory task, Fletcher and co-workers ( [20]; see also Ref. [83]) found a specific decrease in left prefrontal activity (BA 46) at encoding as the demands of the secondary task increased. More recently, Iidaka et al. [37] found that divided attention at encoding reduced blood flow to the left prefrontal and inferior temporal cortices, although it minimally affected the encoding-related activity of the left hippocampus. Taken together, these studies provide support for the view that divided attention interferes with the ability of the prefrontal cortex to carry out the semantic, elaborative processing necessary for successful episodic encoding.

Event-related potentials (ERPs), which measure neural activity that is time-locked to the processing of an individual stimulus, also have demonstrated differential neural activity associated with successful episodic encoding. Specifically, words later retrieved on a subsequent test of explicit memory typically elicit more positive-going potentials at encoding than words later forgotten (see Refs. [40], [79] for review). Differences between the ERPs for words later recalled and those not recalled have been termed ‘differences based on later memory’ or Dm effects [5], [62], [63]. Although Dm effects involve multiple separate components, the most prominent of these are a transient posterior positive wave in the latency range of 300–800 ms and a sustained frontal positivity beginning at about the same time and lasting longer. These components are sensitive to different types of encoding instructions and therefore, may index different cognitive processes. For example, studies have shown that the transient posterior positive wave is more predictive of successful recall for orthographically distinctive words that are studied under rote memorization instructions [17], [18], whereas the sustained frontal positive wave is generally more predictive when words are studied under instructions that emphasize elaborative strategies such as semantic association, semantic verification or imagery [19], [62], [91], [94]. These results suggest that the transient posterior positivity may represent item-specific evaluation and processing of distinctive information, whereas the sustained frontal positivity may represent inter-item associative encoding.

Although ERPs have not yet been used to examine episodic encoding in relation to attention, evidence suggests that both Dm effects index effortful processes that would be influenced by the demands of a secondary task. Fabiani and co-workers related the transient posterior positive wave to the P300 or P3b wave of the ERP, a centro-parietal positivity that follows the detection of task-relevant events [13], [66]. The P3b is sensitive to attentional manipulation and indeed, may primarily index the amount of attention allocated to processing a stimulus [21], [31]. For example, in a target detection task, the amplitude of the P3b is reduced in proportion to the degree to which attentional resources are diverted by a secondary task [96]. Some have argued, however, that the transient memory-related wave is not equivalent to the P3b because this Dm effect has a more widespread distribution [22], [31] and is sensitive only to subsequent memory rather than to perceptual processing in general [62]. One way to reconcile these different interpretations is to consider the possibility that under certain circumstances, the amount of attention deployed in the processing of an item may predict subsequent memory for that item. Stimuli that are physically distinctive may elicit an orienting response that increases item-specific attention, and when elaborative strategies are not employed, this increase may be sufficient to influence and predict subsequent memory. However, elaborative strategies also require the sustained allocation of processing resources. Thus, it is also likely that attention is a necessary condition for the semantic, organizational processing associated with the sustained frontal positivity.

In the present study, we varied attention at encoding by having participants attend only to a series of visually-presented words (focused attention) or divide their attention between the words and an auditory-motor task (divided attention). As in neuroimaging studies of divided attention [37], we varied attentional demands by using easy and difficult versions of the secondary task. ERPs at encoding were averaged both as a function of subsequent memory performance and as a function of attention at encoding. These two averages provide somewhat different information about the encoding process. Averaging as a function of subsequent memory performance provides specific information about those encoding processes that are critical for successful storage in episodic memory. Averaging as a function of attention provides information about the general encoding mode, or neurocognitive set, during conditions of reduced attentional resources, as well specific information about the stages of processing where reductions in attention influence encoding.

We categorized our ERP findings on subsequent memory performance derived from both free recall and remember–know recognition tests. We chose to use both types of test as the basis for selective averaging at encoding for three reasons. Firstly, in previous studies, Dm effects were generally largest and most reliable when based on free recall, smaller and less reliable when based on recognition, and essentially undetectable when based on implicit memory tests [45], [55], [61], [64]. These findings suggest that Dm effects differentially index the conscious, controlled processes that predict successful episodic encoding. By this view, the weak relationship between Dm effects and recognition in these studies may have occurred because retrieval success on recognition tasks can arise from both conscious, controlled processes, which are associated with measurable Dm effects, and unconscious, automatic processes, which are not. This view also would predict, however, that robust Dm effects could be found when based on recognition performance if items retrieved on the basis of conscious recollection (R) and familiarity (K) were averaged separately at encoding.

It is therefore surprising that Smith [84] found no significant ERP differences at encoding between subsequently remembered items and subsequently known items, even though R and K responses could be dissociated at retrieval [14]. Investigators have disputed whether R and K responses index categorically different cognitive processes [29], [39], [76] or whether they simply differ in terms of trace strength (e.g. Refs. [12], [36]), however all appear to agree that R responses involve the conscious recollection of the encoding context. Thus, if memory-related ERPs index processes related to episodic encoding, one would predict that they would differentiate items on the basis of subsequent R and K responses, either qualitatively — if they represent different cognitive processes, or quantitatively — if they exist as points along a unitary dimension. A more recent ERP study, however, found that younger adults showed greater sustained frontal positivity for subsequently remembered items than item subsequently known or missed [23]. Older adults did not show this differentiation, perhaps because of a failure to use elaborative encoding strategies, a deficit in attentional resources, or more lenient retrieval criteria. Thus, a second goal of the present study is to evaluate further whether R and K involve differentiable neural activity and whether ERP components associated with these responses are influenced by changes in attentional resources.

Finally, although successful recall and ‘remembering’ are both thought to arise from successful episodic retrieval [95], free recall and recognition tests have inherently different retrieval demands and are sensitive to different encoding processes. Whereas free recall benefits from relational processing that strengthens inter-item associations and facilitates strategic retrieval, recognition benefits more from item-specific processes that enhance the distinctiveness and discriminability of the individual item [16], [49]. With regard to R responses, some studies have emphasized their dependence on the type of elaborative, associative encoding that also benefits free recall [26]. Others have focused on the relationship of R responses to processing of salient or distinctive attributes of the individual item, which would largely benefit recognition [74]. The most parsimonious view is that both relational and distinctive item-specific processing contribute to the elaborative encoding that results in an R response. Therefore, in the present study, rather than give participants any specific encoding instructions that might result in preferential relational or item-specific processing, we simply encouraged participants to memorize the items. Yet, we averaged the encoding ERPs on the basis of whether the item was later recalled and remembered, remembered but not recalled, known, or missed. If posterior and frontal memory-related components index item-specific and relational processing, respectively, these components may be differentially sensitive to episodic encoding predictive of R items that can only be recognized, versus R items that can be both recalled and recognized.

In summary, we hypothesized that we would find a series of components in the ERPs recorded at encoding that would be sensitive to both the level of attention at study and the level of conscious recollection at retrieval. With regard to the effects of memory, we predicted that a posterior transient wave would be related to item-specific processing contingent on initial conscious apprehension of the incoming information, and that a sustained frontal wave would be related to subsequent elaborative processing. This prediction would be borne out by greater posterior positivity for both subsequently recalled and/or remembered items than missed items, but greater frontal positivity only for items that were both recalled and remembered. With regard to the effects of attention, we predicted attenuation of the sustained frontal positivity under dual-task relative to single-task conditions, regardless of the difficulty of the secondary task. We expected, however, that the more difficult secondary task would affect initial conscious apprehension of the item, and therefore would also attenuate the earlier transient posterior wave. Finally, because our recordings were more widespread over the scalp than in previous ERP studies of this type, an additional goal of this study was to explore other aspects of the encoding waveform that might be related to subsequent memory or attention.

Section snippets

Participants

Twenty young participants (mean age, 28 years; range, 22–37 years; nine female) were tested. All were right-handed, native English-speakers, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no history of neurological disorder. All participants gave informed consent prior to the experiment. Data from three participants were lost due to corrupted data storage and data from two participants were discarded because of excessive skin potential artifacts. As a result, ERP averages were obtained from 15

Memory performance

Memory performance as a function of divided attention is depicted in Table 1. The percentage of items recalled in the free recall test declined when attentional resources were diverted from memorizing the words (F(2,28)=55.4, P<0.0001). Similarly, R responses declined systematically with the increased demands of the secondary task, regardless of how the probability of R responding was estimated. Although raw K hits corrected for false alarms did not show an effect of attention, a main effect of

Overview

The grand mean ERPs at all electrode sites are shown in relation to subsequent memory in Fig. 1 and in relation to the level of attention at encoding in Fig. 2. These ERPs were derived from the same data and therefore, the mean across the four memory categories (recalled/remembered, remembered only, known, missed) at a given time point from Fig. 1 is approximately the same as the mean across the three encoding conditions at the same time point in Fig. 2. Although the overall means are the same,

Overview

The present study used the event-related potentials (ERPs) to delineate a sequence of cerebral processes that occur when a word is encoded into episodic memory and to determine how attention affects this sequence. The amount of attention available for encoding was manipulated using a secondary task with two levels of difficulty. Reduced attention to the encoding task impaired episodic memory, as measured by either free recall or the proportion of items recognized with conscious recollection of

Conclusions

Analysis of the ERPs associated with attention and subsequent memory revealed a sequence of components that represent cognitive processes underlying the decoding and encoding of verbal information into episodic memory. We have proposed the following interpretation of these components (see Table 2). Following initial perceptual analysis of the word stimulus (P120, N180), the item is selected as task-relevant and becomes available for conscious processing (P200, P280). The subsequent

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Medical Research Council and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. We thank Brigitte Boucher for assistance in data acquisition, Patricia Van Roon and Dana Roitberg for assistance in data analysis, Vincent Choi and Shaun Vince for assistance in programming, and Amelia Kaplan for assistance in editing. We are also grateful to Claude Alain, Leun Otten, and Robert West for valuable comments on earlier versions of this article. Various

References (99)

  • E. Halgren et al.

    Intracerebral potentials to rare targets and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. II. Medial, lateral and posterior temporal lobe

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1995)
  • D. Karis et al.

    ‘P300’ and memory: individual differences in the von Restorff effect

    Cogn. Psychol.

    (1984)
  • H.J. Neville et al.

    Event-related brain potentials during initial encoding and recognition memory of congruous and incongruous words

    J. Mem. Lang.

    (1986)
  • K.A. Paller et al.

    Neural correlates of encoding in an incidental learning paradigm

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1987)
  • K.A. Paller et al.

    Brain responses to concrete and abstract words reflect processes that correlate with later performance on a test of stem-completion priming

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1987)
  • K.A. Paller et al.

    ERPs predictive of subsequent recall and recognition performance

    Biol. Psychol.

    (1988)
  • T.W. Picton et al.

    Cephalic skin potentials in electroencephalography

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1972)
  • T.W. Picton et al.

    The correction of ocular artifacts: a topographic perspective

    Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (2000)
  • M.I. Posner et al.

    Attentional networks

    Trends Neurosci.

    (1994)
  • A.P. Rudell et al.

    The recognition potential and conscious awareness

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1996)
  • M.D. Rugg et al.

    An electrophysiological comparison of two indices of recollection

    J. Mem. Lang.

    (1998)
  • M.E. Smith et al.

    Attenuation of a sustained visual processing negativity after lesions that include the inferotemporal cortex

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1988)
  • W. Sommer et al.

    Human brain potential correlates of face encoding into memory

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1991)
  • L.G. Ungerleider et al.

    ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (1994)
  • Y.G. Abdullaev et al.

    Time course of activating brain areas in generating verbal associations

    Psychol. Sci.

    (1997)
  • Guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature

    J. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1991)
  • J.B. Brewer et al.

    Making memories: brain activity that predicts how well visual experience will be remembered

    Science

    (1998)
  • R. Cabeza et al.

    Imaging cognition: an empirical review of PET studies with normal subjects

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (1997)
  • R.M. Chapman et al.

    Memory processes and evoked potentials

    Can. J. Psychol.

    (1981)
  • P.E. Compton et al.

    A cognitive-anatomical approach to attention in lexical access

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (1991)
  • F.I.M. Craik et al.

    Aging and cognitive deficits: the role of attentional resources

  • F.I.M. Craik et al.

    The effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory

    J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.

    (1996)
  • F.I.M. Craik et al.

    In search of the self: a positron emission tomography study

    Psychol. Sci.

    (1999)
  • F.I.M. Craik et al.

    Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory

    J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.

    (1975)
  • W. Donaldson

    The role of decision processes in remembering and knowing

    Mem. Cogn.

    (1996)
  • E. Donchin et al.

    Precommentary: is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (1988)
  • E. Düzel et al.

    Event-related brain potential correlates of two states of conscious awareness in memory

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1997)
  • J. Englekamp et al.

    Human Memory: A Multimodal Approach

    (1994)
  • M. Fabiani et al.

    P300 and recall in an incidental memory paradigm

    Psychophysiology

    (1986)
  • M. Fabiani et al.

    Encoding processes and memory organization: a model of the von Restorff effect

    J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.

    (1995)
  • P.C. Fletcher et al.

    Brain systems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory: an in vivo study in humans

    Brain

    (1995)
  • J.D.E. Gabrieli et al.

    Functional magnetic resonance imaging of semantic memory processes in the frontal lobes

    Psychol. Sci.

    (1996)
  • J.M. Gardiner

    Functional aspects of recollective experience in recognition memory

    Mem. Cogn.

    (1988)
  • J.M. Gardiner et al.

    Maintenance rehearsal affects knowing, not remembering; elaborative rehearsal affects remembering, not knowing

    Psychonom. Bull. Rev.

    (1994)
  • J.M. Gardiner et al.

    How level of processing really influences awareness in recognition memory

    Can. J. Psychol.

    (1996)
  • J.M. Gardiner et al.

    Attention and recollective experience in recognition memory

    Mem. Cogn.

    (1990)
  • T. Grunwald et al.

    Verbal novelty detection within the human hippocampus proper

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1998)
  • J.L. Hicks et al.

    Remember–know judgments can depend on how memory is tested

    Psychonom. Bull. Rev.

    (1999)
  • E. Hirshman et al.

    The role of decision processes in conscious recollection

    Psychol. Sci.

    (1998)
  • Cited by (141)

    • Event-related potentials of episodic encoding after traumatic brain injury in older adults

      2021, Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      In young adults, the P200 amplitude has previously been demonstrated to increase with attentional resources requirement involved in semantic organizational strategies (Blanchet et al., 2007). Mangels et al. (2001) also documented that the amplitude of the P200 component was reduced when encoding words in divided attention, confirming the involvement of the P200 component in attentional processes. The amplitude of P200 has also been associated with successful encoding in full attention in aging (Tellez-Alanis and Cansino, 2004).

    • Offset-related brain activity in the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex promotes long-term memory formation of verbal events

      2021, Brain Stimulation
      Citation Excerpt :

      It could be that, at least in some participants, those encoding processes were yet to complete before associative encoding could initiate in the VLPFC. Interestingly, EEG studies showed that frontal subsequent memory effects reflecting episodic binding start to emerge 1000 ms post-stimulus and follow earlier modulations related to meaning-based and item-specific processing (e.g., Refs. [36,37]). We cannot test this hypothesis directly with the current dataset because response times provide only coarse information on encoding times.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text