American children's use of electronic media in 1997: A national survey

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(00)00064-2Get rights and content

Abstract

Within a weighted, nationally representative sample of 2902 children, differences in electronic media use by age and sex were examined. The data collected were part of the University of Michigan's Child Development Supplement (CDS) to the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID), and included two 24-h time-use diaries, one from a weekday and one from a weekend day. Children and their parents reported the titles of television programs, videotapes, and electronic games (both computer and platform) that the children used. These titles were coded by genre, and differences in total time and in time spent with specific genres within each medium were examined. Main effects of age and sex on total use and use by genres are reported. Among other results, oldest boys were found to spend more time watching sports programs and playing electronic sports games while the oldest girls spend more time watching relationship dramas and not a large amount of time playing video games. The results indicate the diverging media interests of boys and girls. Use patterns may also suggest a lack of availability of appropriate electronic games for older girls.

Introduction

Over the course of the 20th century, broadcast and electronic media became an increasingly important part of the environments in which children grew up. As the 21st century begins, the variety of options available on television and the range of other media activities available are increasing almost exponentially. In the majority of American homes, children have 50–100 television channels as well as video tape players; many also have video game equipment and computers that support interactive software and that provide access to the Internet (Rideout, Foehr, Roberts, & Brodie, 1999). Children spend an average of about 3 h a day with television Huston & Wright, 1997, Rideout et al., 1999, and video and computer games are rapidly becoming a frequent activity for many children. Both theory and popular speculation award these media an important role in children's development, but solid information about who uses them and what they use is scarce.

In this article, we describe age and gender differences and similarities in the patterns of electronic media use (television and video/computer games) in a large nationally representative sample of children ages 0 to 12. A core assumption in this research is that the genre and content of media used by children are critical to understanding their role in children's lives. This assumption is supported by a large body of evidence from television research indicating that educational programs can teach academic and prosocial skills, while viewing general entertainment is associated with lower levels of school readiness and academic performance Anderson et al., in press, Huston et al., in press, Huston & Wright, 1997. Moreover, viewing preferences for particular television genres are quite stable over time (Tangney & Feshbach, 1988).

Computers and video games allow interaction, problem solving, and challenge — all qualities that many people think make them more stimulating than the “passive” processes alleged to be involved in television viewing. Yet, because such games and software range from cognitively simple perceptual–motor activities to highly abstract, complex, and difficult problems, it is important to differentiate them by content. Despite the fact that interactive electronic media surround many American children from birth onward, there is relatively little information about very young children's patterns of use of media other than television, or about age differences in early and middle childhood use. However, the more extensive television literature may provide hints as to what will be learned about interactive media use.

Both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies show that television viewing time increases with age from infancy to around age 6, declines at school entry, then increases again into early adolescence, peaking around 10–12 Comstock, 1991, Huston et al., 1999, Timmer et al., 1985. In one investigation, 2- and 3-year-olds spent considerable time in “secondary” viewing (that is, viewing that accompanied another activity) of general audience programs, probably because they spent time near adults who were viewing, but such secondary viewing declined with age (Wright & Huston, 1995). Genres of programs viewed also change with age. Educational program viewing peaks around age 4; cartoon viewing increases to about age 5, then levels off Funk et al., 1997, Huston et al., 1999, Huston et al., 1990. By the early years of middle childhood, situation comedies are typically the most popular shows (Condry, 1989).

Children's use of computers and video games also changes as they mature. As with television, changes in media-use habits as children grow older appear to result from changes in use opportunities and from cognitive and social developmental changes (Huston et al., 1992). Analyses of longitudinal time-use data for children from ages 2 through 7 showed that video game play increased with age, particularly for boys (Huston et al., 1999). In a cross-sectional analysis of a nationally representative sample, Rideout et al. (1999) reported that 2–7-year-olds spent an average of 40 min a day using computers for games and other purposes; 8–18-year-olds averaged 1 h and 40 min. Buchman and Funk (1996) found that total time playing interactive games decreased as a function of age for fourth through eighth graders.

Unfortunately, most studies do not provide information about the content or type of game or activity. In one study of 4th through 8th graders, younger children were more likely to prefer educational games than were older children (Buchman & Funk, 1996). Among the 8–18-year-olds surveyed by Rideout et al. (1999), only about one-fourth of their computer time was devoted to games; the remainder was spent on Internet uses and work or study tasks.

Previous research shows small, but fairly consistent gender differences in total television viewing, although these change with age. During the preschool years, boys are more frequent viewers than girls, particularly of cartoons and action adventure programs Huston et al., 1990, Singer & Singer, 1981, Wright & Huston, 1995. That difference continues at least until late childhood McKenzie et al., 1992, Ridley-Johnson et al., 1984, Timmer et al., 1985. Patterns of gender differences are less consistent for adolescents Brown et al., 1990, Timmer et al., 1985. On the whole, however, adolescent boys watch more cartoons, action adventure, and sports programs than girls (Comstock, 1991).

By contrast, there are large and consistent gender differences in computer use that begin as early as ages 3 or 4 (Huston et al., 1999). Boys use computers more than girls do, particularly to play video and computer games Funk et al., 1997, Greenfield, 1994, Huston et al., 1999, Rideout et al., 1999. Boys also tend to monopolize computer-use time in the classroom, particularly in periods of uncontrolled access Cassell & Jenkins, 1998, Kinnear, 1995. Many explanations have been proposed, including the characteristics of boys' social networks, advertising, content of games, parental socialization practices, and classroom management strategies by teachers, as well as a growing general cultural expectation of gender-typed usage.

The few studies that have included information about game content demonstrate that boys and girls have different preferences. Gailey (1996) observed game playing in a small sample of urban children (N=21) ages 6 to12; boys preferred violent action games, and girls preferred spatial relations games. In a survey of nine hundred 4th through 8th grade children, girls preferred educational games or those containing cartoon fantasy violence, while boys preferred sports games and those containing realistic human violence Buchman & Funk, 1996, Funk et al., 1997. Gender differences are probably influenced by the pervasive masculine themes of speed and fighting in the vast majority of video games Johnson & Swoope, 1987, Kinder, 1996, Kubey & Larson, 1990.

The goal of the present study was to describe children's patterns of television and interactive game use by age and gender in recently collected data. Despite the volume of literature on television use, available studies generally have one of two weaknesses: poor quality measures of viewing, or small, convenience samples (Huston & Wright, 1997). With the exception of the large-scale survey by Rideout et al. (1999), investigations of computer use suffer from these same weaknesses. The data used for this study suffer from neither of these problems. Our data come from the Child Development Supplement (CDS) to the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID), a large-scale investigation designed to provide a range of assessments of a nationally representative sample of approximately 3000 children ages 0 to 12 years old.

The method in our study was a detailed time-use diary in which respondents reported all of their activities; media were not given special emphasis over other activities, as they are in methods based on media-use diaries or other forms of self-reported media use. Most important for our purposes, respondents recorded titles of television programs and video games when either were used, permitting analyses by program genre and game type.

Section snippets

The panel study of income dynamics

Beginning in 1968, the PSID is an ongoing panel study focusing primarily on the transfer of social and economic capital within families. In 1997, additional data concerning on PSID children and their families were collected via the CDS. All families participating in the PSID with children under 12 years old were asked to complete the CDS. A total of 2380 families agreed to participate, yielding a sample of 3562 children (Hofferth & Sandberg, 1999). The subsample used here includes all

Total minutes of primary television viewing

Because of the large sample size, an alpha level of .01 was used for all statistical tests. An alpha level of .05 was considered marginally significant.

Discussion

With respect to television use, the present data support the general findings in the existing literature. Previous studies of television use have found that preschool children (3–5-year-olds) and older elementary school children (9–12-year-olds) spend more time with television than do young school-aged children (6–8-year-olds) and very young children (0–2-year-olds; Timmer et al., 1985). This pattern is also evident in the present data. Very young children watch whatever their adult caregiver

Acknowledgements

Funding for this research was provided by Grant No. R01HD33474 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development to the University of Michigan's Institute for Social Research, by Grant No. 3797 from the Hogg Foundation for Mental Health to the first author, and by Grant No. 98-1911-98 from the William T. Grant Foundation to the first three authors. This research was conducted at the Center for Research on the Influences of Television on Children (CRITC), University of Texas at

References (28)

  • R Ridley-Johnson et al.

    Correlates of children's television viewing: expectancies, age, and sex

    Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology

    (1984)
  • Anderson, D. R., Huston, A. C., Schmitt, K., Linebarger, D., & Wright, J. C. (in press). Adolescent outcomes associated...
  • J.D Brown et al.

    The influence of new media and family structure on young adolescents' television and radio use

    Communication Research

    (1990)
  • D.D Buchman et al.

    Video and computer games in the '90s: children's time commitment and game preference

    Children Today

    (1996)
  • J Cassell et al.

    Chess for girls? Feminism and computer games

  • G Comstock

    Television and the American child

    (1991)
  • J Condry

    The psychology of television

    (1989)
  • J.B Funk et al.

    Children and electronic games in the United States

    Trends in Communication

    (1997)
  • C.W Gailey

    Mediated messages: gender, class, and cosmos in home video games

  • P.M Greenfield

    Video games as cultural artifacts

    Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology

    (1994)
  • Hofferth, S., & Sandberg, J. (1999). American children's use of time in 1997. Unpublished Manuscript. Ann Arbor:...
  • Hofferth, S. L., & Sandberg, J. F. (in press). How American children use their time. Journal of Marriage and the...
  • Huston, A. C., Anderson, D. R., Wright, J. C., Linebarger, D. L., & Schmitt, K., (in press). Sesame Street viewers as...
  • A.C Huston et al.

    Big world, small screen: the role of television in American society

    (1992)
  • Cited by (115)

    • Covid-19 pandemic and its impact on increasing the risks of children's addiction to electronic games from a social work perspective

      2021, Heliyon
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although there are some opinions that see that electronic games contribute to the development of the child in play, increase his skills, activate his areas of thinking, enrich his imagination and activate them in a wider direction, and push his abilities to growth and wide awareness, but at the same time they carry a lot of harm to the child, Especially on his physical, psychological, mental and behavioral health, and on the overall patterns of his culture in general, due to the negative and dangerous consequences of many electronic games, because a large proportion of electronic games depend on entertainment and enjoyment of killing others and destroying their property and assaulting them without right (Fromme, 2003; Olson et al., 2007). These games teach children and adolescents the methods of committing a crime, its arts, and tricks, and develop in their minds the abilities of bullying, aggression, and the consequences of the crime, These abilities are acquired through excessive persistence in playing these games, it is worth noting that these games may be more harmful than violent television films because they are characterized by an interactive character between them and the child, and they require the child to assume the aggressive character that he plays and practices in the electronic game, so many psychologists blame electronic games as being among one of the reasons that lead to the emergence of some behavioral disorders in the child and the problems of violence and bullying in the school environment, social isolation or introversion, in addition to some health problems, and the problem of poor academic achievement of the child (Wright et al., 2001; Papastergiou, 2009). So children games, especially electronic games in our modern era are not a simple matter as many people imagine it, Game may reach a dangerous stage if it does not improve its choice, as it may have a bad or negative impact on the child's physical and psychological health when the child uses his game and practices it in a wrong way, or if it is the tools used in play are above the level of the child's age and mind.

    • Gaming- a bane or a boon-a systematic review

      2019, Asian Journal of Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      In 2017, a survey of gamers showed that those aged 18 years and above represented 72% of the population, their average age being 35 years (Essential Facts About the Computer and Video Game Industry, 2017). Video games are played by children as young as three years with teenage boy gamers consistently being more than girls (Wright et al., 2001). 13- or 14-years old gamers of both genders played an average of 17 h in a week.

    • Psychological paradox of game software trial

      2018, Information and Management
      Citation Excerpt :

      People usually derive fun and enjoyment from game software that can be evaluated in terms of aesthetics or sensory experience (e.g., visual and musical appeal) [24], which can fulfill individuals’ hedonic desires for social value, playfulness, and accomplishment [24,27,28]. Second, compared with utilitarian software [29], game software requires emotional involvement from users to achieve the unique values it brings. To achieve this goal, an individual must personally experience the game software [24]; in other words, the trial process and its incurred user experiences are critical in influencing post-trial game software evaluation.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text