Public and private self-consciousness as specific psychopathological features

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00077-XGet rights and content

Abstract

Public self-consciousness (PUBSC) and private self-consciousness (PRISC) represent aspects of dispositional self-directed attention. The aim of the present study was to investigate whether four clinical groups, namely patients with a diagnosis of social phobia, panic disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder or bulimia nervosa, and normal controls differentiated on the basis of their PUBSC and PRISC scores. Results indicated that both PUBSC and PRISC are extremely elevated in patients with social phobia. Patients with panic disorder and patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder score somewhere between social phobics and normal controls on PUBSC and PRISC scales. Bulimics had significantly higher PUBSC values than both controls and patients with panic disorder, but had lower values than social phobics. Compared to normal controls, bulimics showed no elevated PRISC scores. In addition, contrary to the other groups, PUBSC and PRISC seem to be unrelated in bulimics. Thus, PUBSC and PRISC appear to be specific psychopathological features for German females when comparing groups with different mental disorders and normal controls.

Introduction

Fenigstein, Scheier and Buss (1975) proposed a distinction between self-consciousness and self-awareness. Self-awareness refers to a state of self-directed attention, while self-consciousness refers to dispositional self-directed attention. The dispositional tendency of a person to direct attention to the self can be further divided into public self-consciousness (PUBSC) and private self-consciousness (PRISC). PUBSC refers to the concern with self-presentation and with others’ view of oneself (e.g., worrying about making a good impression). In contrast, PRISC refers to the thoughts and feelings about oneself (e.g., constantly examining one’s motives).

Ingram (1990a) supposed that self-consciousness is—like self-focused attention in general—a non-specific psychopathological feature. In accordance with this view, no differences between agoraphobic patients, patients with affective disorders, patients with anorexia and patients with bulimia were found with respect to PUBSC in a study by Belfer and colleagues (cf. Striegel-Moore, Silberstein & Rodin, 1993). Results from a study by Belfer and Glass (1992) lent no support to the assumption that self-consciousness is a psychopathological feature at all. In this study, no differences were found for either PUBSC or PRISC between agoraphobics, simple phobics and normal controls.

This finding is contrary to the results of Sanz and Avia (1994) who compared extreme groups selected from a sample of male and female Spanish students. In this study four groups were formed: (1) socially anxious subjects, (2) depressed subjects, (3) both socially anxious and depressed subjects and (4) controls having low scores for social anxiety and depression. No difference was found between groups with respect to PUBSCS, although controls showed lower values of PRISC than the other three groups. Ingram (1990b) used an analogue design to compare four groups consisting of male and female students from the USA. Each group showed a different pattern of anxiety and depression scores: (1) high anxiety, (2) high depression, (3) both high anxiety and high depression and (4) both low anxiety and low depression. Compared to the fourth group, the first three groups showed higher scale values for both PRISC and PUBSC.

The association of PUBSC and PRISC to different forms of anxiety was not only investigated in studies comparing clinical groups or extreme groups. In some studies quantitative features of anxiety disorders were set in relation to PUBSC and PRISC. Belfer and Glass (1992) found weak correlations between PRISC and fear of losing control and between PRISC and fear of negative social reactions when anxious for both controls and agoraphobics. In addition, for agoraphobics PRISC was also correlated weakly with the level of perceived autonomic arousal. Agoraphobic fears were also found to be correlated with both PUBSC and PRISC in a Swedish study (Saboonchi & Lundh, 1997). In an Icelandic study (Smári, Clausen, Hardarson & Arnarsln, 1995), however, no correlation above r=0.20 was found between agoraphobic fears and both PUBSC and PRISC.

Hope and Heimberg (1988) carried out a correlational study with a total of 44 male and female patients who received the primary diagnosis of social phobia according to DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980). Although restrictions of variance for the anxiety measures used have to be considered in this highly selected sample, the authors were able to identify correlations between social anxiety and PUBSC and PRISC, respectively. Overall, this study indicated that PUBSC seems to be more relevant for social phobia than PRISC. Correlational studies with non-clinical samples, however, revealed inconsistent results with respect to the association between PRISC and social anxiety. An Australian (Monfries & Kafer, 1993) and a Swedish study (Saboonchi & Lundh, 1997) investigating males and females revealed an association between social anxiety and both PUBSC and PRISC. In contrast, both an Icelandic (Smári et al., 1995) and a Dutch study (Bögels, Alberts & de Jong, 1996), also investigating males and females, showed a correlation between social anxiety and PUBSC, but no association between social anxiety and PRISC.

There are few studies investigating the association between features of eating disorders and PUBSC and PRISC, respectively. Here, studies comparing clinical groups or extreme groups to healthy controls yielded results similar to those of studies correlating eating related cognitions to PRISC and PUBSC. Striegel-Moore et al. (1993) compared bulimic patients, non-clinical subjects with elevated levels of disturbed eating and normal controls with respect to PRISC and PUBSC. Bulimic patients and subjects with high scores on eating disorder scales had significantly higher scores of PUBSC than normal controls. No differences between groups were found with respect to PRISC. A study by Geissner, Bauer and Fichter (1997) indicated that compared to normative data, PUBSC is also elevated in anorexic patients. In this study PRISC was not assessed. Blanchard and Frost (1983) found in a sample of female students that concern about dieting was correlated only weakly with PUBSC (r=0.30, p<0.001) and was not correlated to PRISC (r=0.10, not significant). In addition, assuming a positive intercorrelation between PUBSC and PRISC and using formulae according to Steiger (1980), a reanalysis of this data revealed that PUBSC is correlated significantly higher with regard to dieting than PRISC. A study by Striegel-Moore et al. (1993) revealed similar findings for scales assessing body dissatisfaction or pathological eating attitudes in a non-clinical sample of females.

To summarise, there is some evidence that PUBSC is relevant in patients with eating disorders, but not PRISC. In reviewing the results, it remained unclear, however, to what extent features of PUBSC and PRISC are characteristic of patients with different anxiety disorders. Since clear empirical findings are essential for the development of theoretical concepts about cognitive specificity in mental disorders, it was the aim of the present study to investigate whether four clinical groups, namely patients with a diagnosis of social phobia, panic disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder or bulimia nervosa, and normal controls might be differentiated on the basis of their PUBSC and PRISC scores. In order to minimise variability within groups, only women were included in the present study.

Section snippets

Subjects

A total of 295 women participated in the present study. Subjects for the clinical subgroups (n=255) were patients of an inpatient treatment center. These patients volunteered for the study in the context of the diagnostic phase preceding the planning of treatment. Normal controls were recruited by advertisements in local newspapers. Due to the wide range of age and educational level of the patients, no restrictions on these variables were set for normal controls. Participants were screened with

Results

The groups were significantly different with respect to age (F(4.278)=16.27, p<0.01). Therefore, the association between age and PUBSC and PRISC was estimated. No correlation between age and PUBSC was found (r=−0.09, not significant), whereas the correlation between age and PRISC was highly significant (r=0.16, p<0.01). However, because the association is very weak, age seems not to be a relevant covariate for the following data analysis.

For the complete sample as well as for every subgroup,

Discussion

Our results represent a replication of former findings along with the discovery of several new findings. For example, we were able to replicate the finding of Striegel-Moore et al. (1993) that bulimics show elevated values of PUBSC but not of PRISC when compared to normal controls. The corresponding finding of the present study is not only in line with former empirical results but is also concordant with a theoretical consideration concerning binge eating. According to the escape theory (

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Christoph Dornier Foundation of Clinical Psychology (CDK/14/95).

References (29)

  • J Cohen

    Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences

    (1988)
  • A Fenigstein et al.

    Public and private self-consciousness: assessment and theory

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1975)
  • S-H Filipp et al.

    Der Fragebogen zur Erfassung dispositionaler Selbstaufmerksamkeit (SAM-Fragebogen)

    (1989)
  • E Geisner et al.

    Videogestützte Konfrontation mit dem eigenen körperlichen Erscheinungsbild als Behandlungselement in der Therapie der Anorexia nervosa

    Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie

    (1997)
  • Cited by (20)

    • The neuropsychology of self-reflection in psychiatric illness

      2014, Journal of Psychiatric Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Consistent with these clinical descriptions, numerous empirical studies have found elevated levels of self-focused thought in depression, social anxiety, and social phobia (Ingram et al., 1987; Woodruff-Borden et al., 2001). For instance, self-report measures of self-reflection have been used to associate depression, social anxiety, and social phobia with heightened self-reflection and self-consciousness (Hope and Heimberg, 1988; Jostes et al., 1999; Smith et al., 1985). Additionally, a sentence completion task has also been used to demonstrate increased self-focused thought in depression and anxiety disorders (Ingram et al., 1987; Woodruff-Borden et al., 2001).

    • Examination of the trait facets of the five-factor model in discriminating specific mood and anxiety disorders

      2012, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The GSP group had higher self-consciousness scores compared to all other clinical groups. This finding is consistent with past studies that have found public self-consciousness to be higher in patients with social phobia compared to patients with PD/A (Jostes et al., 1999; Saboonchi et al., 1999), early community-based studies on NEO-PI-R rated self-consciousness and social phobia (Bienvenu et al., 2004), and more recent research examining laboratory-based ratings of self-consciousness in social phobia (George and Stopa, 2008; Higa et al., 2008). Finally, these findings are consistent with leading clinical cognitive-behavioral models of social phobia that highlight the importance of self-consciousness in the persistence of the disorder (Clark and Wells, 1995; Rapee and Heimberg, 1997; Wells, 1997).

    • Interpretations of and memory for bodily sensations during public speaking

      2009, Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry
    • Personality traits and anxiety symptoms: The multilevel trait predictor model

      2007, Behaviour Research and Therapy
      Citation Excerpt :

      According to expectancy theory, individuals high in NES are prone to experiencing discomfort in social situations and tend to avoid them. Consistent with Reiss’ model, NES shows clear specificity to SP, as patients with SP have elevated scores on measures of NES relative to normal controls and to patients with panic disorder or OCD (Ball, Otto, Pollack, Uccello, & Rosenbaum, 1995; Jostes, Pook, & Florin, 1999; Saboonchi, Lundh, & Ost, 1999). In sum, previous research has linked anxiety disorders to a number of different traits, including broad personality dimensions and clinical traits.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Deceased

    View full text