Elsevier

Psychiatry Research

Volume 105, Issues 1–2, 15 December 2001, Pages 87-95
Psychiatry Research

Sleep-EEG in borderline patients without concomitant major depression: a comparison with major depressives and normal control subjects

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00330-4Get rights and content

Abstract

The link between borderline personality disorder (BPD) and the affective disorders remains controversial. The aim of this study was to examine the relationships between BPD and major depression (MD) from the perspective of sleep parameters and to contribute to the characterisation of the sleep-EEG in BPD. We compared 20 off-medication BPD in-patients without co-existing MD with 20 sex- and age-matched MD patients without BPD and 20 sex- and age-matched control subjects. BPD patients had a greater prevalence of drug or alcohol abuse and suicide attempts than MD patients. MD patients had higher scores on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS). Both BPD and MD patients had less total sleep time, more prolonged sleep onset latency, and a greater percentage of wakefulness than control subjects. BPD patients and control subjects had more stage 2 sleep than MD patients. BPD patients had a longer duration of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, and less stage 3, stage 4 and slow wave sleep than MD patients and control subjects. REM latency did not differentiate the three groups. BPD and MD patients shared sleep-continuity characteristics, but sleep architecture differentiated the two groups. BPD patients with a past history of MD had more wakefulness and less slow wave sleep than BPD patients without a history of MD; other sleep parameters, age, sex and HDRS scores were not statistically different in the two BPD subgroups. Although BPD and MD may coexist, the present study offers more arguments favouring the concept that they are not biologically linked and that BPD patients with depressive symptoms often experience an affective syndrome different from that in MD patients without BPD, in terms of quality and duration of symptoms and of the biological substrate.

Introduction

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) manifests a wide variety of symptoms. BPD has been associated with several pathological conditions such as epilepsy, affective disorders, schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and atypical psychoses (Andrulonis et al., 1982, Post et al., 1982, Schulz et al., 1989). Although the hypothesis of a relationship with epilepsy is difficult to support, there is evidence of an excess of electroencephalographic (EEG) slow activity, which can indicate brain dysfunction, in patients with BPD (De la Fuente et al., 1998). Other methodologies, such as positron emission tomography, have also shown the existence of brain abnormalities in BPD (De la Fuente et al., 1997).

The link between BPD and the affective disorders is controversial (Korzekwa et al., 1993, Coid, 1993, Rogers et al., 1995). Arguments for a link between BPD and affective disorders (Schulz et al., 1989) are contrasted with those that suggest the two disorders to be independent (Soloff et al., 1989, Benson et al., 1990). Gunderson and Phillips (1991) conclude that the two disorders may coexist, but are not etiologically related. Some authors have suggested that depression associated with BPD is distinguished from depression without BPD by the quality and duration of symptoms (Westen et al., 1992, Coid, 1993, Rogers et al., 1995). Moreover, other authors have proposed that the depressed symptoms in BPD might have a biological substrate that is distinct from that in depressive illness without concomitant BPD characteristics (Krishnan et al., 1984, Korzekwa et al., 1993, Kavoussi et al., 1993, De la Fuente and Mendlewicz, 1996). Pharmacological studies have shown differences in medication response between BPD and MD patients. Conventional antidepressants have been reported to lead to poor response and even worsening in BPD (Cole et al., 1984, Soloff et al., 1989).

The dexamethasone suppression test (DST) and the thyrotropin-releasing hormone stimulation test (TRH-ST), which have proved valuable in the study of affective illness, have also been studied in BPD (Korzekwa et al., 1991, De la Fuente and Mendlewicz, 1996). In some of these studies, positive findings with the DST and TRH-ST seem to be more related to an Axis I diagnosis of MD than to BPD itself (Soloff et al., 1991). The only study evaluating the DST and the TRH-ST in BPD patients without a co-existing diagnosis of MD (De la Fuente and Mendlewicz, 1996) provided no evidence for a biological link between BPD and the MD.

Another approach to the putative affective nature of BPD is the study of sleep-electroencephalography (EEG), which has been useful in the biological characterisation of depressive and other psychiatric syndromes (Benca et al., 1992). To our knowledge, there have been at least eight studies on the sleep-EEG in BPD. Most have disregarded or not taken into account the existence of a concomitant Axis I MD in the BPD patients. Bell et al., (1983) described the ‘characteristic short REM latency reported in MD’ in a group of eight Research Diagnosis Criteria (RDC)-primary major depressive patients with borderline symptoms and 11 primary depressive patients without borderline symptoms. McNamara et al. (1984) found sleep continuity disturbances, greater rapid eye movement (REM) activity and shortened REM latency in 10 depressed borderline women and MD patients compared with controls. Reynolds et al. (1985) compared 20 retrospective and prospective BPD (MD was not an exclusion criterion and most patients had RDC diagnoses of depression) to 10 MD patients and to controls; they found similar REM latency values in both patient groups. King et al. (1987) compared 18 BPD (eight with current or past history of MD) patients with 16 MD patients and controls; BPD patients could not be distinguished from controls, whereas MD patients had shortened REM latencies compared with the other two groups. Lahmeyer et al. (1988) found similar EEG-sleep abnormalities in 17 BPD patients (eight also had RDC diagnoses of MD) and 20 MD patients; stage 1 sleep was lower and sleep efficiency was higher in the BPD group. In these five studies, the sleep-EEG perturbations could be more directly linked to a concomitant MD than to BPD itself (Lahmeyer et al., 1989).

Only three studies have clearly considered the existence of MD in their BPD patients. Akiskal et al. (1985) compared 24 non-MD BPD patients (not diagnosed with the Diagnostic Interview of Borderlines) to 30 non-BPD MD patients, 16 patients with other personality disorders, and 14 controls (the non-MD BPD patients had significantly higher scores on the Beck Depression Inventory than the major depressives); they found similar REM latencies in the BPD and MD groups. In the study of Benson et al. (1990), 18 BPD patients with and without a present or past history of affective disorder were compared to each other and to controls. The three groups could not be distinguished in terms of REM latency; the BPD group had less total and delta sleep and more time awake after sleep onset than controls. Battaglia et al. (1993) studied 10 ‘never-depressed’ BPD patients and controls; they found reduced REM latency in the BPD group.

Taken together, the phenomenology and duration of the depressive episodes in BPD, along with their poor response to conventional antidepressants and the findings of some TRH-ST, DST and EEG sleep studies, provide suggestive evidence against the existence of a biological link between BPD and MD. Furthermore, the depressive symptoms observed in BPD patients might not have the same biological substrates as those found in MD patients.

The aim of our without Major Depression study was to further examine the relationships between BPD present and MD patients from the perspective of sleep-EEG parameters and to contribute to the characterisation of the sleep-EEG in BPD patients without Major Depression. Specifically, we wanted to test the hypothesis that, as already proposed by us in studies with the DST and the TRH-ST, sleep-EEG patterns in BPD without Major Depression patients and MD patients would be different, therefore not giving arguments favouring the existence of a biological link between BPD and the MD nor between the substrates of the depressive symptoms in BPD and non-borderline MD.

Section snippets

Subjects

Twenty in-patients (14 women and six men, mean age=32.40; range 21–45) who met DSM-III-R criteria for BPD (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) as established in a clinical interview and with a score of at least 7 on the Diagnostic Interview for Borderlines (DIB; Gunderson et al., 1981) were recruited. All the patients were also assessed with the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia, lifetime version (SADS-L; Spitzer and Endicott, 1978). The reasons for their hospitalisation

Results

Table 1, Table 2, Table 3 present clinical and EEG-sleep characteristics of the sample. All the patients in the three groups were free of primary sleep disorder. Two MD patients had past histories of BPD not confirmed by the DIB or by DSM-III-R. BPD patients showed a greater lifetime prevalence of drug or alcohol abuse and of suicide attempts than MD patients. MD patients had significantly higher scores on the HDRS. The lifetime prevalence of major depression was not statistically different in

Discussion

Several differences in sleep-EEG patterns were found between non-depressed BPD patients and MD patients. The parameters of continuity TST, SOL, and percentage of wakefulness differentiated both BPD and MD patients from controls. Sleep-architecture parameters clearly differentiated BPD from MD patients. BPD patients and controls had more stage 2 than MD patients. BPD patients had less SWS and longer REM sleep duration than MD patients and controls. The differences in delta sleep between BPD and

References (51)

  • M. Kerkhofs et al.

    Sleep-EEG recordings in depressive disorders

    Journal of Affective Disorders

    (1985)
  • H.W. Lahmeyer et al.

    EEG sleep, lithium transport, dexamethasone suppression, and monoamine oxidase activity in borderline personality disorder

    Psychiatry Research

    (1988)
  • C.F. Reynolds et al.

    Depression in borderline patients: a prospective EEG sleep study

    Psychiatry Research

    (1985)
  • C.F. Reynolds et al.

    Sleep, gender and depression. An analysis of gender effects on the electroencephalographic sleep of 302 depressed outpatients

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1990)
  • J. Adamson et al.

    Sleep of dry alcoholics

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1973)
  • H.S. Akiskal et al.

    The nosologic status of borderline personality. Clinical and polysomnographic study

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (1985)
  • Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

    (1987)
  • P.A. Andrulonis et al.

    Borderline personality subcategories

    Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease

    (1982)
  • M.H. Benca et al.

    Sleep and psychiatric disorders. A meta-analysis

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1992)
  • R.D. Cartwright

    Rapid eye movement sleep characteristics during and after mood disturbing events

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1983)
  • E.F. Coccaro et al.

    Familial correlates of reduced central serotoninergic system function in patients with personality disorders

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1994)
  • W.J. Coid

    An affective syndrome in psychopaths with borderline personality disorder

    British Journal of Psychiatry

    (1993)
  • I. Feinberg et al.

    Stage 4 sleep in schizophrenia

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1969)
  • D.E. Giles et al.

    REM latency threshold values in depression and normal controls, sensitivity and specificity data

    Sleep Research

    (1987)
  • J.G. Gunderson

    The relatedness of borderline and schizophrenic disorders

    Schizophrenia Bulletin

    (1979)
  • Cited by (48)

    • The sleep phenotype of Borderline Personality Disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis

      2017, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      It appears that BPD may have an independent association with a range of sleep problems (many of them objectively measured) not accounted for by concomitant depression (or other psychiatric disorders). This conclusion is supported by converging evidence from clinical studies utilising BPD patients with and without comorbid Axis I disorders (e.g., Battaglia et al., 1993; De la Fuente et al., 2001), and larger studies controlling for the effects of depression symptoms and substance dependence on BPD-sleep associations (Harty et al., 2010; Oltmanns et al., 2014; Selby, 2013). BPD and depressed patients appear to share similar objective sleep disturbances, which could indicate an overlap in biological processes.

    • Sleep and borderline personality disorder: A review

      2013, Asian Journal of Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      The primary studies on sleep of BPD patients tended to find evidence for differences/similarities in biological basis of BPD and affective disorders (i.e. depression). Although the results of studies in this field are very controversial, generally it can be concluded that BPD patients with depression have shorter slow wave sleep duration and longer REM sleep latency than BPD patients without depression (Asaad et al., 2002; La Fuente et al., 2001, 2004). According to the several studies’ findings, the history of childhood abuse can increase the risk of BPD in adulthood (Elzy, 2011; Fossati et al., 1999; Igarashi et al., 2010; Tyrka et al., 2009; Widom et al., 2009; Wolke et al., 2012).

    • Sleep and dreaming in patients with borderline personality disorder: A polysomnographic study

      2012, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      The difference in REM density might imply that co-morbid PTSD has a specific effect on sleep, an observation that needs further clarification. This difference might explain why previous sleep studies in borderline patients that included a considerable proportion of patients with co-morbid PTSD failed to find elevated REM densities in these patients (Benson et al., 1990; De la Fuente et al., 2001; Philipsen et al., 2005). Differences regarding dream recall frequency were small and not significant.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text