Paternal socio-psychological factors and infant attachment: The mediating role of synchrony in father–infant interactions

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00123-6Get rights and content

Abstract

The present research explored the role of synchrony as a mediator of the relationship between marital satisfaction and infant attachment, and between depressive symptoms and infant attachment. Fifteen father–infant and mother–infant dyads were videotaped in face-to-face interactions. Videotaped interactions were coded for frequency of synchronized interactions. Quality of attachment was assessed several months later using the Waters’ Attachment Q-sort (AQS). Tests of mediation revealed that synchrony accounts for the relation between marital quality and infant attachment to fathers, and between depressive symptoms and level of attachment to mothers. Finally, demographic variables associated with work and family role strain were explored as potential correlates of marital dissatisfaction and depressive symptoms.

Introduction

The formation of secure attachments during infancy has been associated with a broad range of positive developmental outcomes, including increased social competency (Bohlin, Hagekull, & Rydell, 2000; Booth, Rose-Kraanor, & Rubin, 1991; Sroufe, 1983), intellectual curiosity (Arend, Gove, & Sroufe, 1979), persistence in problem-solving (Frankel & Bates, 1990; Matas, Arend, & Sroufe, 1978), and more symbolic play (Slade, 1987). Although most research has focused on the developmental outcomes of infant–mother relationships, secure infant–father attachments have also been linked to positive developmental outcomes (Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984; Lamb, Hwang, Frodi, & Frodi, 1982; Main & Weston, 1981). Furthermore, infants securely attached to both parents demonstrate higher levels of competency compared to infants securely attached to only one parent (Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984).

The positive outcomes associated with attachment security have resulted in numerous investigations into potential developmental antecedents. Maternal sensitivity, for example, has been explored extensively in relation to the formation of infant–mother attachment relationships. Infants with more sensitive mothers are more likely to develop secure attachments to their mothers (see de Wolff & van Ijzendoorn, 1997, for a recent meta-analysis). The strength of this relationship, however, has been questioned (Seifer, Schiller, Sameroff, Resnick, & Riordan, 1996). Unfortunately, the maternal sensitivity construct has been operationalized and measured in various ways by different investigators. For the purpose of the present research, sensitivity was defined in terms of interactional synchrony, or the “extent to which an interaction appears to be reciprocal and mutually rewarding” (Isabella, Belsky, & von Eye, 1989, p. 13). Isabella et al. (1989) reported that, while the interactions between mothers and infants with secure attachments, were characterized by disproportionately more frequent synchronous exchanges, those of insecurely attached infants were disproportionately asynchronous (i.e., one-sided, unresponsive, or intrusive). However, whether the frequency of synchronized interactions is similarly related to the formation of secure attachments to fathers has not yet been investigated.

Most infants do form attachments to their fathers (see review by Lamb, 1976b). Early research on father–infant relations focused primarily on the interaction styles of fathers compared to mothers (Lamb, 1975, Lamb, 1977, Nash, 1965, Parke & Tinsley, 1987), and infants’ preferences for one parental gender over another (Lamb, 1976a, Lamb, 1976c; Spelke, Zelazo, Kagan, & Kotelchuck, 1973). However, interest subsequently turned to the importance of infant–father attachment security in relation to early developmental outcomes (Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984, Lamb et al., 1982, Main & Weston, 1981). The positive outcomes have resulted in a few investigations into potential antecedents. Similar to the maternal research, one such antecedent has included parental sensitivity (see van Ijzendoorn & de Wolff, 1997, for a recent meta-analysis).

Although a weak association has been found between paternal sensitivity and infant attachment security, the findings have been inconsistent and the measures of sensitivity have differed across studies (Cox, Owen, Henderson, & Margand, 1992; Easterbrooks & Goldberg, 1984, Notaro & Volling, 1999, Volling & Belsky, 1992). When assessed in terms of emotional supportiveness and quality of assistance during a problem-solving task, a relationship between sensitivity and infant attachment was not demonstrated (Easterbrooks & Goldberg, 1984). However, when assessed within the context of play, a composite variable that included global ratings of sensitivity, warmth, stimulation and activity level was found to significantly predict infant–father attachment security (Cox et al., 1992). In addition to the inconsistent findings and measures, only a few studies conducted with fathers have included comparable data from mothers (Cox et al., 1992, Notaro & Volling, 1999).

The maternal research indicates that certain socio-psychological factors can interfere with a mother’s ability to engage in synchronous interactions. Depressed mothers, for example, tend to exhibit either intrusive (i.e., overstimulating) or withdrawn (i.e., understimulating) styles of interaction (Cohn, Campbell, Matias, & Hopkins, 1990; Field, Healy, Goldstein, & Guthertz, 1990; Jones et al., 1997; Jones, Field, Hart, Lundy, & Davalos, 2001). These styles of interaction have been associated with delays in early socioemotional and cognitive development (Field, 1992, Gelfand & Teti, 1990; Zahn-Waxler, Iannotti, Cummings, & Denham, 1990), and are similar to the types of interactions associated with the development of insecure attachments (Lyons-Ruth et al., 1990, Lyons-Ruth et al., 1986; Radke-Yarrow, Cummings, Kuczynski, & Chapman, 1985; Teti, Gelfand, Messinger, & Isabella, 1995).

Furthermore, the depression does not have to be clinical depression. Maternal reports of depressive symptoms have also been found to influence maternal sensitivity (Bettes, 1988; Donovan, Leavitt, & Walsh, 1998), synchrony in mother–infant behavioral states (Field, 1992), and early socioemotional and cognitive development (Field, 1992). Maternal reports of depressive symptoms have also been linked to the development of insecure infant attachments (Dawson et al., 2001). Even depression that has been experimentally induced has been found reduce the mothers’ ability to elicit positive responses from their infants (Zekowski, O’Hara, & Wills, 1986).

The depression is more likely to have a negative impact on the mother–infant relationship when combined with additional socio-psychological stressors (Lyons-Ruth et al., 1990; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, NICHD, 1999; Sameroff, Seifer, Baldwin, & Baldwin, 1993; Seifer, 1995, Shaw & Vondra, 1993). Maternal depression has been associated with stressors such as conflict regarding whether or not to return to work (Donovan et al., 1998). In addition, a common correlate of maternal depression is marital dissatisfaction (e.g., Coyne, Burchill, & Stiles, 1991; Downey & Coyne, 1990, Gotlib & Whiffen, 1989, Lyons-Ruth et al., 1990), which has also been associated with reduced maternal sensitivity (Cox, Owen, Lewis, & Henderson, 1989; Donovan et al., 1998, Goldberg & Easterbrooks, 1984, Howes & Markman, 1989, Isabella & Belsky, 1985). Thus, maternal depression and marital dissatisfaction may each contribute to reduced levels of maternal sensitivity.

The influence of socio-psychological factors, such as depression and marital quality, on fathers’ interactions with infants has received less attention. However, the quality of parental relations has been suggested to have a greater impact on fathers’, compared to mothers’, relationships with infants (Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984; Belsky, Youngblade, Rovine, & Volling, 1991; Cox et al., 1989, Goldberg & Easterbrooks, 1984, Lamb & Elster, 1985, Volling & Belsky, 1992). Higher levels of marital satisfaction in fathers has been associated with more positive thoughts and feelings toward their infants (Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984, Corwyn & Bradley, 1999). Furthermore, because the quality of the marital relationship appears to be so strongly related to the nature of the father–infant relationship, some researchers have proposed that fathers may view marriage and fatherhood as one and the same (Corwyn & Bradley, 1999, Furstenberg & Cherlin, 1991).

Marital dissatisfaction in fathers has been associated with stressors such as work-family role conflict (Barling, 1986) and financial strain (Elder, Conger, Foster, & Ardelt, 1992). The role of fatherhood is still stereotyped as the primary economic provider (Cowan & Cowan, 1987). At the time many fathers are becoming parents, they are also engaged in establishing new careers (Cowan & Cowan, 1987). Compared to mothers, fathers still receive little support from employers with regard to family caregiving responsibilities, and paternity leave is still rarely available (Cowan & Cowan, 1987). Thus, the processes that underlie work-family role conflict for fathers may differ from those of mothers (Corwyn & Bradley, 1999). In general, fathers who find themselves with greater caregiving responsibilities that conflict substantially with their responsibilities related to work, or with their role as economic provider, may experience increased levels of marital dissatisfaction (Corwyn & Bradley, 1999). Financial strain may also have a more negative affect on the fathers’ level of marital quality, compared to the mothers’, and may affect the father–infant relationship more adversely than the mother–infant relationship (Elder et al., 1992).

The role of depression on fathers’ interactions with their infants has received much less attention. However, the effects of depression may be greatest when combined with poor marital quality. In one study, fathers with mild depression touched and interacted with their infants less frequently and provided their infants with less affection compared to nondepressed fathers (Zaslow, Pederson, Cain, Suwalsky, & Kramer, 1985). In addition, paternal depression was found to be inter-correlated with marital conflict (Zaslow et al., 1985). In another study, depressed fathers interacted with their infants similarly to nondepressed fathers (Field, Hossain, & Malphurs, 1999). However, none of the fathers were legally married, although the majority resided with their infants’ mothers (Field et al., 1999). It is possible that conflict within a marriage may be more distressing, since the relationship between the parents is likely to be more significant, than in a nonmarital relationship.

The influence of socio-psychological factors, such as depression and marital quality, on fathers’ interactions with infants has received less attention compared to the maternal research. However, marital quality has been suggested to have a greater impact on fathers’, compared to mothers’, relationships with their infants (Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1991, Cox et al., 1989, Goldberg & Easterbrooks, 1984, Volling & Belsky, 1992). Furthermore, based on the limited research available, while depression and marital dissatisfaction may co-occur in fathers (Zaslow et al., 1985), it is unclear whether paternal depression per se, may have the same negative effect on father–infant interactions (Field et al., 1999) as maternal depression has on mother–infant interactions (Cohn et al., 1990, Field et al., 1990, Jones et al., 1997, Jones et al., 2001).

Section snippets

Present research

The present research had two main purposes. The first was to explore the relations among paternal socio-psychological factors, synchrony and infant attachment. Interactional synchrony was expected to mediate the relation between marital quality and infant attachment for both fathers and mothers. Based on previous research, level of marital satisfaction in fathers (Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1984, Belsky et al., 1991, Cox et al., 1989, Goldberg & Easterbrooks, 1984,

Participants

Participants were 30 parents (15 fathers, M=31.13 years, SD=6.63; mothers, M=27.87 years, SD=4.27, and their infants, M=6.3 months, SD=1.69; 8 males, 7 females). Participants were recruited through the assistance of local paediatricians, daycare facilities, newspaper advertisements, and the psychology department parent–infant subject pool. The sample was predominately white (96%), lower-middle SES (M=2.93, SD=1.03 on the Hollingshead, 1975), and diverse in terms of education level (7% some

Results

Results are presented in two sections. In the first section, analyses related to synchrony and attachment are presented, including: (a) inter-correlations among predictor variables (marital satisfaction, depressive symptoms) and frequency of synchrony, and (b) mediational analyses conducted separately on the fathers’ and mothers’ data with synchrony as a mediator. In the second section, correlations among demographic stressors and predictor variables are presented separately for fathers and

Discussion

Secure attachments to fathers, as with mothers, has been associated with a broad range of positive developmental outcomes. However, while antecedents and potential barriers have been studied extensively in relation to the formation of infant–mother attachments, relatively little research has been conducted with regard to the development of infant–father relationships. The purpose of the present research was to explore the relations among paternal socio-psychological factors, interactional

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Purdue Research Foundation (PRF) summer 2001 and 2002 research grants. The author would like to thank the fathers, mothers and infants who participated in this research and to acknowledge Carrie Arnold, Lisa Federspiel, Holly Frier, Tera Haberkorn and Jenifer Schutte for their assistance with data collection and video coding.

References (76)

  • G.K. Baruch et al.

    Consequences of fathers’ participation in family work: Parents’ role strain and well-being

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1986)
  • J. Belsky et al.

    The Pennsylvania infant and family development project: I. Stability and change in mother–infant and father–infant interaction in a family setting at 1, 3, and 9 months

    Child Development

    (1984)
  • J. Belsky et al.

    Assessing performance, competence, and executive capacity in infant play: Relations to home environment and security of attachment

    Developmental Psychology

    (1984)
  • J. Belsky et al.

    Q-sort security and 1st-year nonmaternal care

    New Directions for Child Development: Child Care and Maternal Employment

    (1990)
  • J. Belsky et al.

    The Pennsylvania infant and family development project, II: The Development of reciprocal interaction in the mother–infant dyad

    Child Development

    (1984)
  • J. Belsky et al.

    Patterns of marital change and parent–child interactions

    Journal of Marriage and the Family

    (1991)
  • B.A. Bettes

    Maternal depression and motherese: Temporal and intonational features

    Child Development

    (1988)
  • G. Bohlin et al.

    Attachment and social functioning: A longitudinal study from infancy to middle childhood

    Social Development

    (2000)
  • C.L. Booth et al.

    Relating preschoolers’ social competence and their mothers’ parenting behaviors to early attachment security and high-risk status

    Journal of Social and Personal Relationships

    (1991)
  • J.F. Cohn et al.

    Face-to-face interactions of postpartum depressed and nondepressed mother–infant pairs at 2 months

    Developmental Psychology

    (1990)
  • R.F. Corwyn et al.

    Determinants of paternal and maternal investment in children

    Infant Mental Health Journal

    (1999)
  • Coyne, J. C., Burchill, S. A. L., & Stiles, W. B. (1991). An interactional perspective on depression. In C. R. Synder &...
  • Cowan, P. C., & Cowan, P. A. (1987). Men’s involvement in parenthood: Identifying the antecedents and understanding the...
  • M.J. Cox et al.

    Prediction of infant–father and infant–mother attachment

    Developmental Psychology

    (1992)
  • M.J. Cox et al.

    Marriage, adult adjustment, and early parenting

    Child Development

    (1989)
  • M.S. de Wolff et al.

    Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment

    Child Development

    (1997)
  • G. Downey et al.

    Children of depressed parents: An integrative review

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1990)
  • M.A. Easterbrooks et al.

    Toddler development in the family: Impact of father involvement and parenting characteristics

    Child Development

    (1984)
  • G.H. Elder et al.

    Families under economic pressure

    Journal of Family Issues

    (1992)
  • R. Feldman et al.

    Developmental Psychology

    (1999)
  • T. Field

    Interaction behaviors of primary versus secondary caretaker fathers

    Developmental Psychology

    (1978)
  • T.M. Field

    Infants of depressed mothers

    Development and Psychopathology

    (1992)
  • T. Field et al.

    Behavior-state matching and synchrony in mother–infant interactions of nondepressed versus depressed dyads

    Developmental Psychology

    (1990)
  • T. Field et al.

    “Depressed” mothers’ perceptions of infant vulnerability are related to later development

    Child Psychiatry & Human Development

    (1996)
  • T.M. Field et al.

    “Depressed” fathers’ interactions with their infants

    Infant Mental Health Journal

    (1999)
  • K.A. Frankel et al.

    Mother–toddler problem-solving: Antecedents in attachment, home behavior, and temperament

    Child Development

    (1990)
  • Furstenberg, F. F., & Cherlin, A. J. (1991). Divided families: What happens to children when parents part. Cambridge,...
  • W.A. Goldberg et al.

    The role of marital quality in toddler development

    Developmental Psychology

    (1984)
  • Cited by (57)

    • Depression and playfulness in fathers and young infants: A matched design comparison study

      2018, Journal of Affective Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      One possible mechanism for the familial transmission of risk is through its negative effects on parenting and the father-child relationship (Kane and Garber, 2009; Nath et al., 2016; Sethna et al., 2015, 2012). So far, evidence indicates that paternal depression is associated with less optimal father-infant interactions with a few exceptions (Field et al., 1999; Lundy, 2002). For example, in comparison to non-depressed fathers, depressed fathers are less responsive in free-play with their 3-month old infants (Parfitt et al., 2013), tend to engage in fewer activities, such as, outdoor play, singing or reading to their children aged 9-months (Paulson et al., 2006), touch their 4-month old infants less frequently during routine tasks (Zaslow et al., 1985), and are less engaged during free play at 3 months and 12 months (McElwain and Volling, 1999; Sethna et al., 2015).

    • Marital conflict and parental responses to infant negative emotions: Relations with toddler emotional regulation

      2015, Infant Behavior and Development
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, we anticipate that both marital conflict and parents’ distressed responses to their infants’ negative emotions will be independently and directly related to their toddlers’ negative and flat/withdrawn affect, and in addition, parent's distressed responses to their infants’ negative emotions will both mediate and moderate the relation of marital conflict to toddler's negative and flat/withdrawn affect. Based on past research, we further expect that these hypotheses will hold for both mothers and fathers, but based on the fathering vulnerability hypothesis, spillover effects from marital conflict to distressed parental responses may be greater for fathers than mothers (Goeke-Morey & Cummings, 2007; Lundy, 2002; Owen & Cox, 1997). Participants were 125 couples who were recruited to participate in a longitudinal study that followed couples from the third trimester of pregnancy before the birth of their first child until that child reached 7 years of age, 96 of whom remained for the entire study.

    • Examining antecedents of infant attachment security with mothers and fathers: An ecological systems perspective

      2015, Infant Behavior and Development
      Citation Excerpt :

      Mothers may also use compensatory mechanisms whereby problems in the marriage actually promote more sensitive parenting, perhaps to protect children from stress (Belsky, Youngblade, Rovine, & Volling, 1991). Fathers, on the other hand, may be more susceptible to spill-over effects, suggesting that fathers whose marital relationships are struggling may have more difficulty in their parenting role (Cummings, Goeke-Morey, & Raymond, 2004; Lundy, 2002). In addition, studies have found that fathers who are more satisfied in their marriages are more likely to interact positively with their infants (Coiro & Emery, 1998) and are more invested in their children (Corwyn & Bradley, 1999).

    • Trajectories of parenting behavior and maternal depression

      2013, Infant Behavior and Development
      Citation Excerpt :

      Compared to nondepressed mothers, depressed mothers tend to be less sensitive, more withdrawn and self-occupied (Field, 1984; Tronick & Cohn, 1989). This type of parenting, after controlling for demographic variables such as income and maternal education, was related to lower scores on cognitive and verbal development (Murray, 1992; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1999), insecure attachment (Lundy, 2002; Mills-Koonce, Gariepy, Sutton, & Cox, 2008; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1999), child behavior problems (Ghodsian, Zajicek, & Wolkind, 1984), and reduced left frontal brain activity (Dawson et al., 2003). The relations between maternal depression, parenting, and child outcomes are complex since neither depression nor maternal parenting are static entities but are likely to change over time.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text