Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 38, Issue 5, 1 May 2000, Pages 712-721
Neuropsychologia

Distinctive patterns of memory function in subgroups of females with Turner syndrome: evidence for imprinted loci on the X-chromosome affecting neurodevelopment

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00118-9Get rights and content

Abstract

X-monosomy is a form of Turner syndrome (TS) in which an entire X chromosome is missing. It is usually assumed that neuropsychological deficits in females with TS result from insufficient dosage of gene products from alleles on the sex chromosomes. If so, then parental origin of the single X chromosome should be immaterial. However, if there are imprinted genes on the X chromosome affecting brain development, neuropsychological development will depend on the parental origin of the single X chromosome. We contrasted verbal and visuospatial memory in females with a single paternal X chromosome (45,Xp) and those with a single maternal X (45,Xm). Neither group showed any impairment on immediate story recall; if anything, performance was above control levels. Groups did not differ on a measure of delayed recall. However, when delayed recall was considered after adjusting for level of immediate recall, 45,Xm females showed enhanced verbal forgetting relative to controls over a delay. On the Rey figure, both groups were poor at copying the figure, but, after adjusting scores for initial copy score and strategy, only the 45,Xp females showed disproportionate forgetting relative to controls. We propose there may be one or more imprinted genes on the X chromosome that affect the development of lateralised brain regions important for memory function.

Introduction

Approximately 30% of the human genome is expressed only in the brain, yet we understand relatively little about how such influences operate. Two approaches are available to neuropsychologists who wish to investigate genetic influences on brain development. The first is conventional behavioural genetics, which involves identifying individuals with particular neurocognitive or behavioural characteristics and considering how far genetic relatedness predicts phenotypic similarity. The second is the molecular approach in which one starts with individuals who have a known genetic anomaly and looks for neurodevelopmental consequences [30]. In this paper, we adopt the latter approach, in a study documenting aspects of the neuropsychological phenotype in Turner syndrome (TS), a chromosomal disorder in which all, or a substantial part, of one X chromosome is missing. We argue that there are subtle impairments of retention, with the profile depending on the parental origin of the single X chromosome. We conclude that a phenomenon known as genomic imprinting affects genes on the X chromosome that are involved in the development of memory systems.

TS is associated with distinctive physical characteristics, the most reliable of which are short stature and low production of female sex hormones. It has been known for many years that there is a distinctive intellectual profile in TS [27]. Typically, verbal ability is in the normal range, but visuospatial skills are impaired. More recently, other characteristics have been described, including memory deficits, attentional problems, motor impairment, and difficulties in social relationships. However, although the incidence of such difficulties is increased in TS, the correlation between neuropsychological profile and genotype is far from perfect, and several authors have commented on the wide phenotypic variation seen in this syndrome [2], [23]. Structural brain correlates of TS have only recently been investigated using neuroimaging, and have revealed abnormalities in the parietal lobes [20], [25]. One study [20] also found that women with TS had reduced volume of the hippocampus, caudate, lenticular and thalamic nuclei, a finding that is consistent with reports of deficits in long-term memory [19], [23].

At first glance it is surprising that loss of an X chromosome should have neurodevelopmental consequences, because in normal females, the second X chromosome is inactivated very early in embryonic development [38]. This X-inactivation is random: in some cells the X inherited from the mother is inactivated, in others the X inherited from the father. The Y chromosome has few functions other than primary sex determination, so if X-inactivation did not occur, then normal males, with XY karyotype, would have only half as much gene product as females. X inactivation ensures equal dosage of gene product for both sexes. Thus normal females, normal males, and females with TS, all have only one activated X-chromosome. Why then should females with TS differ from those with two sex chromosomes? The conventional explanation is the haploinsufficiency hypothesis, which attributes the phenotype of TS to insufficient dosage of gene product. This explanation implicates genes on the X chromosome that are the exception to the general rule of X-inactivation [39]. Molecular studies have confirmed that there is a region of the X chromosome (the pseudoautosomal region) that escapes inactivation, so genes from both X chromosomes are active in normal females. In this region, there is evidence that males have functionally equivalent genes on the Y chromosome, so both males and females have a double dose of gene product. More recently, other regions of the X chromosome that escape inactivation have been discovered [6]. According to the haploinsufficiency hypothesis, the phenotypic abnormalities in TS, including the cognitive profile, can be explained in terms of a deficiency of gene products from a region of the X-chromosome that escapes inactivation. Adverse effects could be direct effects of genes on brain development, or more indirect consequences of the hormonal deficiencies that result from the genetic deficit.

Recently, we suggested that a phenomenon known as genomic imprinting might play a role in determining the phenotype associated with TS. Genomic imprinting is another mechanism whereby one member of a pair of alleles is inactivated, but in this case, the inactivation is not random, but is determined by the parental origin of the chromosome. The phenomenon of imprinting is a relatively recent discovery, which has been investigated both experimentally and clinically, with a focus mainly on autosomal rather than sex chromosomes (see Keverne [12] for a review). It is possible to create mice that carry a duplicated gene from one parent, rather than the normal complement of one gene from each parent. Such animals show serious developmental disturbances, reflecting the fact that certain genes are silenced unless they come from a specific parent. Clinically, our understanding of imprinting has been advanced by the study of genetic disorders in which there is deletion on chromosome 15, in the region q11-13. When such a deletion affects the maternally derived chromosome, the result is Angelman’s syndrome, with a phenotype of severe mental handicap, lack of speech, and other physical and behavioural abnormalities. When it is the paternally derived chromosome that is deleted, the very different clinical picture of Prader–Willi syndrome is observed, characterised by hypotonia and hypothalamic dysfunction in the context of mild mental handicap. The relevant region of chromosome 15 has been mapped with gene markers, to reveal a region where only maternal alleles are transcribed, and an adjacent region where only paternal alleles are transcribed. Imprinted genes have distinctive effects on brain development, and Keverne postulated that they might have been important for remodelling of the brain during mammalian evolution.

Imprinted X-linked genes have recently been identified in humans [21] as well as autosomal imprinted genes that are expressed in the brain [12], [18], [28]. We argued that if humans had imprinted genes affecting neurodevelopment on the X chromosome, then we would expect to see neurocognitive differences between females with a single X chromosome (i.e., monosomic TS), depending on the parental origin of the X chromosome [29], [33]. Suppose there were an X-linked gene that was expressed only when inherited from the father. Females with a single maternal X chromosome would lack the relevant gene product, whereas those with a single paternal X chromosome would show normal gene expression. Furthermore, because the single X in Turner syndrome is entirely normal, it is plausible that the actions of X-linked imprinted loci could be associated with sexual dimorphism of equivalent phenotypic features in normal (46,XmY) males and (46, XmXp) females [31]. Thus the imprinted locus hypothesis predicts (i) differences within TS, depending on parental origin of the X chromosome, and, (ii) sex differences in the normal population, for phenotypes which depend on expression of a paternally-derived X-linked gene. This was the pattern of results we reported in a study of social adjustment in TS. Although TS females in general had a somewhat elevated risk of impaired social adjustment, the risk of serious difficulties was substantially greater in those with a maternal X chromosome (45,Xm) than in those with a paternal X chromosome (45,Xp).

Section snippets

Study 1: verbal long-term memory

The imprinted gene hypothesis predicts phenotypic variation within the TS population, and therefore is a promising candidate to account for some of the more variable neuropsychological correlates of the syndrome. As noted above, impairments of long-term verbal memory have been noted in TS, and related to hippocampal abnormalities [20], but only a minority of females have evidence of such impairment [23]. Indeed, on some tests of verbal memory, there is evidence of superior functioning in TS [35]

Females with Turner Syndrome

Once again, we restricted our investigation to monosomic females, but, to increase statistical power we included adults up to the age of 26 years, to give a sample of 46 in group 45,Xm and 20 in group 45,Xp. We again excluded cases with significant neurological disease, or with a verbal IQ below 70.

Control group

We recruited a normally-developing control group consisting of 76 boys and 89 girls aged between 7.5 and 16 years, from the same four schools in Greater London. Children whose prorated verbal IQ was

General discussion

This study of females with TS confirms previous reports of visuospatial perceptual deficits in this population, and indicates that this impairment is characteristic of TS, regardless of parental origin of the X chromosome. This was not true, however, for long-term memory deficits. Previous studies of long-term memory in TS have usually found some impairment, though results have been somewhat inconsistent from study to study [7], [19], [23], [26]. Even where such deficits are reported, it has

Acknowledgements

The research was supported by the Wellcome Trust and the Child Growth Foundation. Compilation of the national register of Turner syndrome was supported by the British Society for Paediatric Endocrinology and by Pharmacia Ltd. Specific assistance was given by Elinore Percy, Sarah Cave, Anne O’Herlihy, Rikki South, Jennifer Smith, Gina Aamodt-Leeper, Catharine Creswell, Rhona McGurk, Rowena James, Paola Dalton, Brian Coppin, Monique Bacarese-Hamilton, Monica Power and David Robinson. We are

References (39)

  • E. Beardsworth et al.

    Assessment of long-term verbal memory in children

    Memory

    (1994)
  • J. Bennett-Levy

    Determinants of performance on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test: an analysis, and a new technique for single-case assessment

    British Journal of Clinical Psychology

    (1984)
  • D.V.M. Bishop

    Language development after focal brain damage

  • D.V.M. Bishop

    Uncommon understanding: development and disorders of language comprehension in children

    (1997)
  • L. Buchanan et al.

    A reexamination of the visuospatial deficit in Turner syndrome: contributions of working memory

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (1998)
  • L. Fenson et al.

    Variability in early communicative development

    Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    (1994)
  • W. Henn et al.

    Mosaicism in Turner’s syndrome

    Nature

    (1997)
  • A. Herlitz et al.

    Gender differences in episodic memory

    Memory and Cognition

    (1997)
  • P. Jacobs et al.

    Turner syndrome: a cytogenetic and molecular study

    Journal of American Human Genetics

    (1997)
  • Cited by (87)

    • Turner Syndrome

      2020, Sperling Pediatric Endocrinology: Expert Consult - Online and Print
    • Sex chromosome aneuploidies

      2018, Handbook of Clinical Neurology
      Citation Excerpt :

      These are typified by considerable difficulty in copying and subsequently remembering complex designs, due to a poor visuospatial memory. Individuals with the syndrome also find it hard to complete a speeded peg-moving task (a motor task with visuospatial demands) because of motor skills difficulties as well as the visuospatial deficits (Bishop et al., 2000). A contributory factor, leading to difficulties in social adjustment, results from deficits in socioperceptual processing.

    • Etiologies underlying sex differences in Autism Spectrum Disorders

      2014, Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology
      Citation Excerpt :

      In that case only males and Xm Turner females do not transcribe that gene (or genes) which could make them vulnerable to ASD. Additionally, verbal and visuospatial memory has also shown to be influenced by imprinting, with better verbal recall scores in Xp than Xm Turner females and better visuospatial memory in Xm than Xp females (Bishop et al., 2000). In concordance with that finding, Xm females are found to have a larger superior temporal gyrus (Kesler et al., 2003), a region used in speech processing.

    • Turner syndrome

      2014, Pediatric Endocrinology: Fourth Edition
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text