Elsevier

Journal of School Psychology

Volume 40, Issue 1, January–February 2002, Pages 85-111
Journal of School Psychology

General article
Phonological Awareness and Beginning Reading in Spanish-Speaking ESL First Graders: Research into Practice

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4405(01)00095-4Get rights and content

Abstract

In the first study, 30 Spanish-speaking English-as-a-second language (ESL) first graders whose families were Latino immigrants and who received all their school instruction in English completed an assessment battery with both Spanish and English measures of phonological awareness, Verbal IQ (VIQ), oral language proficiency, and single-word reading (real words and pseudowords); they also named English alphabet letters. Phonological awareness in Spanish predicted (a) phonological awareness in English and (b) English word reading; thus, phonological awareness may transfer across first and second languages and across oral and written language. English VIQ and oral language proficiency predicted both English and Spanish word reading, but Spanish VIQ and oral language proficiency did not predict English word reading. In the second study, the 4 males and the 4 females with the lowest reading achievement participated in an instructional design experiment in which empirically supported instructional components for teaching beginning reading to monlingual English speakers were included. These components were phonological awareness training (in both Spanish and English), explicit instruction in alphabetic principle (in English), and repeated reading of engaging English text with comprehension monitoring (in English). Both individual students and the group as a whole increased in real-word reading and pseudoword reading beyond the level expected on the basis of their Spanish or English VIQ or oral proficiency. Implications of this research for school psychology practice are discussed, especially the importance of early reading intervention and progress monitoring for Spanish-speaking ESL first graders.

Section snippets

First study: best predictors of beginning reading in spanish-speaking esl students

Given that a child's first language may influence the child's second oral and written language in an additive or subtractive fashion, it is important for school psychologists to have empirical evidence for which assessment measures are the best predictors of oral and written second language development in Spanish-speaking ESL students. Two of the best predictors of beginning reading in monolingual English-speaking beginning readers are letter knowledge and phonological awareness (National

Second study: early intervention as a diagnostic tool

School psychologists who are knowledgeable about empirically supported best practices in early reading instruction for monolingual English-speaking children may be the most able to help the Spanish-speaking ESL beginning readers by translating assessment results into effective intervention. A growing body of research provides empirical support for the effectiveness of early intervention in preventing reading disabilities (e.g., Berninger et al. 2000, Foorman, Francis, Fletcher, Schatschneider,

Research questions

Four research questions are addressed in the research reported in this article. First, is phonological awareness important in the beginning stages of learning to read English when the student's first language is Spanish? Second, can the prior findings of cross-language transfer from phonological awareness in the first language to reading (Durgunoglu et al., 1993) or phonological awareness in the second language (Ciscero & Royer, 1995) be replicated in another sample of Spanish-speaking ESL

Participants

Although there is an influx of immigrants to the metropolitan region where the study took place, no individual school had a critical mass of any one language minority; therefore, each of the schools working with our center had only a few Spanish-speaking ESL first graders. Thus, it was necessary to draw children from different schools and classrooms. First-grade teachers in 10 schools and 15 classrooms in three school districts (urban, suburban, and semirural) that were participating in other

Participants

The 4 lowest achieving males and 4 lowest achieving females participated in a one-to-one tutorial. Their mean word reading achievement prior to the tutorial was nearly two standard deviations below the mean in real-word and pseudoword reading; see Table 4. This study used an instructional design experiment (Brown, 1992) that provided empirically supported instructional components to achieve the desired outcome of improved word reading (see the introduction).

Procedures

Each child received twelve 30-min

General discussion

Cummins (1980) found that pull-out bilingual programs focus on the more visible manifestations of language, which do not carry over to areas other than basic communication. He referred to this level of language proficiency as “basic interpersonal communications skills” (BICS). Within the highly structured milieu of an elementary school, students can appear to have more developed language ability when, in reality, they are functioning within the BICS level of ability. Cummins (1981) questioned

Conclusion

This research focused on only one ESL student group: those who speak Spanish as a first language, are instructed in school in English, are beginning readers in first grade, and live in families that have recently immigrated to the United States. Clearly, more research is needed with different language-speaking ESL groups and with other groups of Spanish-speaking ESL students. However, the two studies reported here do provide some insight into how school psychologists might serve ESL students

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a minority fellowship to the first author as a supplement to a Multidisciplinary Learning Disability Center Grant from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (P 50 33812-03 and -04) for which the last author is Principal Investigator and the fourth author is the statistician. The second author, who was a graduate research assistant, and third author, who was the research coordinator for the Center, developed the Spanish version of the

References (45)

  • V. Berninger et al.

    A three-tier model for assessment-intervention links for preventing and treating reading and writing disability

  • I. Liberman et al.

    Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (1974)
  • F. Vellutino et al.

    The case for early intervention in diagnosing specific reading disability

    Journal of School Psychology

    (1998)
  • M. Adams

    Beginning to readThinking and learning about print

    (1990)
  • Auguste, D., & Hakuta, K. (Eds.). (1998). Educating language-minority children (pp. 21–32). Washington, D.C.: National...
  • V. Berninger

    Process assessment of the learnerGuides for reading and writing intervention

    (1998)
  • Berninger, V. (in press). Best practices in reading, writing, and math assessment-intervention links: A systems...
  • V. Berninger et al.

    Redefining learning disabilitiesMoving beyond aptitude-achievement discrepancies to failure to respond to validated treatment protocols

  • V. Berninger et al.

    A connectionist approach to making the predictability of English orthography explicit to at-risk beginning readersEvidence for alternative, effective strategies

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2000)
  • V. Berninger et al.

    Preventing reading disabilities by assessing and remediating phonemic skills

    School Psychology Review

    (1987)
  • A. Brown

    Design experimentsTheoretical and methodological challenges in creating complex interventions in classroom settings

    The Journal of Learning Sciences

    (1992)
  • Case studies in bilingual educationFirst year report (Federal Grant # G008303723)

    (1985)
  • C. Ciscero et al.

    The development and cross-language transfer of phonological awareness

    Contemporary Educational Psychology

    (1995)
  • Cummins, J. (1980). The construct of language proficiency in bilingual education. Paper presented at the Georgetown...
  • J. Cummins

    The Cross-lingual dimensions of language proficiencyImplications for bilingual education and the optimal age issue

    TESOL Quarterly

    (1981)
  • J. Cummins

    Bilingualism and special educationIssues in assessment and pedagogy

    (1984)
  • J. Cummins

    Interdependence of first- and second-language proficiency in bilingual children

  • R. Diaz

    The impact of bilingualism on cognitive development. Review of Research in Education

    (1983)
  • S. Duncan et al.

    Pre-LAS

    (1986)
  • A. Durgunoǧlu et al.

    Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1993)
  • J. Fitzgerald

    English-as-a-second-language reading instruction in the United StatesA research review

    Journal of Reading Behavior

    (1995)
  • J. Fitzgerald et al.

    About hopes, aspirations, and uncertaintyFirst-grade English-language learners' emergent reading

    Journal of Literacy Research

    (1999)
  • Cited by (116)

    • Early education of dual language learners: An efficacy study of the Nuestros Niños School Readiness professional development program

      2017, Early Childhood Research Quarterly
      Citation Excerpt :

      Further evidence supports the notion that shared language skills could also potentially support literacy development. Several studies in this area suggest that language skills in L1 support literacy in L2 (August & Shanahan, 2006; Cummins, 2005; Dunn Davison et al., 2011; Quiroga et al., 2002). Initial levels of emergent writing also seem to develop in parallel in both languages (Gillanders, Franco, Seidel, Castro, & Méndez, 2016).

    • English Learners’ Performance on a Measure of Dyslexia Risk

      2023, Learning Disabilities Research and Practice
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    The first two authors, bilingual school psychologists and native speakers of both Spanish and English, contributed equally to this research, which was conducted while they were interns in school psychology at the University of Washington; thus, order of authorship is arbitrary. They are continuing their work with Spanish-speaking ESL students in schools in Arizona and California, respectively.

    View full text