Original articles
Impaired modulation of the saccadic contingent negative variation preceding antisaccades in schizophrenia

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00234-1Get rights and content

Abstract

Background: The contingent negative variation (CNV) is considered to reflect prefrontal functioning and can be observed before manual and ocular motor responses. Schizophrenic patients exhibit reduced CNV amplitudes in tasks requiring manual motor responses. A number of studies has also found normal prosaccades, but delayed antisaccades and an augmented rate of erroneous prosaccades during the antisaccade task in schizophrenia. In this study we examined the CNV during pro- and antisaccade tasks in schizophrenic patients and healthy control subjects.

Methods: Data of 17 medicated schizophrenics (ICD-10, F20) and 18 control subjects, matched with patients for age, gender, and education were analyzed. Horizontal pro- and antisaccades were elicited in four blocks, each consisting of 80 trials. Electroencephalogram was recorded from 32 channels with a DC amplifier.

Results: Patients exhibited delayed correct responses and more erroneous prosaccades during the antisaccade task than control subjects, but normal prosaccadic reaction times. In control subjects, the vertex-predominant saccadic CNV was generally larger than in patients, and larger during the anti- than during the prosaccade task. This task-related amplitude augmentation was absent in patients. Analyses of additional components suggested specificity of impaired event-related potential modulation to the saccadic CNV.

Conclusions: In accordance with the presumed prefrontal dysfunction, our results suggest deficient preparation and execution of antisaccades in schizophrenia.

Introduction

According to Fuster 1984, Fuster 1985, Fuster 1989, one of the functions of the prefrontal cortex is the mediation of cross-temporal contingencies for the temporal organization of goal-directed behavioral sequences. This function has been investigated most intensively with delay tasks. In delay tasks, the information provided by a stimulus, which is presented at the beginning of the trial, has to be retained during a delay period in order to be able to select the correct, rewarded response upon presentation of a second stimulus. Unit recordings in primates have revealed delay-related activity in the prefrontal cortex that seems to serve two complementary cognitive functions: working memory and preparatory set Funahashi et al 1993, Fuster 1984, Fuster 1989. Working memory enables the organism not only to retain behaviorally relevant information over time, but also to select an appropriate action on the basis of an internal representation; preparatory set involves the adjustment of the sensory and motor systems before an expected event in order to optimize the reception of stimuli and the anticipated (motor) response (Fuster 1989, p. 163). A rostro-caudal sequencing of motor response preparation seems to take place, because response-related units in the monkey principal sulcus (corresponding to Brodman’s area 46 in humans) increase their firing before units in the premotor and motor cortex (Fuster 1989, p. 103).

The two-stimulus paradigm is a variant of the delay task (Fuster 1989): A warning stimulus (WS), presented at the beginning of the trial, reliably signals the subsequent presentation of an imperative stimulus (IS), which is delayed by some seconds and associated with a certain task (Rockstroh et al 1989). Between WS and IS, a surface-negative potential arises referred to as the contingent negative variation (CNV; Walter 1964). For a number of reasons, the CNV is considered to reflect the delay-related activity of the prefrontal cortex; it is also seen to reflect the activity of additional cortical areas that are recruited by the prefrontal cortex as part of the behavioral structures of delay tasks Fuster 1984, Fuster 1985, Fuster 1989: 1) Between the WS and IS, a surface-negative cortical potential can be recorded at many prefrontal sites of the monkey cortex, including the dorsal and ventral banks of the principal sulcus (Sasaki and Gemba 1991); 2) subdural potentials similar to the CNV can be measured preceding the IS at different prefrontal sites in epileptic patients during presurgical evaluation Hamano et al 1997, Ikeda et al 1996; 3) the CNV amplitude is positively related to the working memory load imposed by a delayed matching-to-sample task (Klein et al 1996).

Slow surface-negative potential shifts have been recorded from the premotor, supplementary motor, motor, and somatosensory cortices of monkeys (Sasaki and Gemba 1991) and humans Hamano et al 1997, Lamarche et al 1995 before prewarned motor responses; they may reflect the aforementioned preparatory adjustment of the sensory and motor systems. These cortical potentials—with the exception of the prefrontal potentials (Rektor et al 1994)—have also been found preceding self-paced hand Hamano et al 1997, Ikeda et al 1994, Neshige et al 1988, Rektor et al 1994 or eye (Sakamoto et al 1991) movements. They are considered to be the source of the “Bereitschaftspotential” (“readiness potential,” RP; Kornhuber and Deecke 1965) that can be measured at the scalp. The RP has been suggested to be at least part of the terminal phase of the CNV (e.g., Brunia 1988; Roesler 1991). Despite this partial overlap between CNV and RP in their cortical generator structures, two arguments suggest that these components should be distinguished: 1) In neurological patients, degeneration of the basal ganglia reduces or abolishes the CNV, but preserves the RP (Ikeda et al 1997), whereas the reverse pattern is seen after decussation of the superior cerebellar peduncle (Ikeda et al 1994). The cerebellum projects to the premotor and motor cortex; the basal ganglia, however, also project to the prefrontal cortex (Kandel et al 1991); 2) the structure of the RP tasks (initiation of a movement) lacks the components of establishment of cross-temporal contingencies or sensory-motor integration that characterize delay tasks.

Saccadic eye movements are typically elicited within the two-stimulus paradigm: A central fixation point is presented as the WS. This is followed a few seconds later by the peripheral cue, which serves as the IS and the saccade goal, in the case of visually guided or “prosaccades.” Averaging time-locked to the onset of the saccade Evdokimidis et al 1992, Everling et al 1997, Klostermann et al 1994, or to the onset of the peripheral cue (e.g., Evdokimidis et al 1996, Gomez et al 1996) reveals surface-negative potential shifts with a topographical maximum at anterior central locations. Unit recordings in primates have revealed that neurons in the prefrontal frontal eye fields (FEF) and the premotor supplementary eye fields (SEF; Schall 1991, Schlag and Schlag-Rey 1987) show increased firing before visually guided saccades. In addition, during the execution of visually guided saccades as compared to a fixation control condition, in human subjects augmented blood flow in the FEF and the supplementary motor area (SMA) have been found in positron emission tomography (PET; Anderson et al 1994, Melamed and Larsen 1979, Petit et al 1993) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI; Darby et al 1996) studies.

During the “antisaccade” task Hallett 1978, Hallett and Adams 1980, subjects are instructed not to look at the cue that is presented in the visual periphery, but to look “voluntarily” in the opposite direction, that is, generate an antisaccade. The initiation of anti- as compared to prosaccades is typically delayed (e.g., Reuter-Lorenz et al 1995), and even normal subjects may happen to glance unconsciously (Mokler and Fischer 1999) at the peripheral cue in a number of trials. For some reason, this task seems to “stress” prefrontal or frontal cortical functions more than the prosaccade task: 1) Patients with large excisions of frontal lobe tissue (Guitton et al 1985), or with circumscribed lesions of the dorsolateral (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al 1991) or ventrolateral (Walker et al 1998) prefrontal cortex, but not of the FEF or the SMA (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al 1991), are impaired at inhibiting erroneous prosaccades during the antisaccade task; 2) patients with FEF lesions, however, exhibit delayed antisaccade initiation (Rivaud et al 1994). This clinical observation is complemented by physiologic results showing increased blood flow in the human FEF and SMA Doricchi et al 1997, Nakashima et al 1994, O’Driscoll et al 1995, Sweeney et al 1996, augmented neuronal firing in the monkey SEF Amador et al 1995, Schlag-Rey et al 1997, and greater slow negative potential shifts at anterior central sites in human subjects Evdokimidis et al 1996, Everling et al 1998 before anti- as compared to prosaccades; 3) inhibition of a peremptory response (looking at the peripheral cue) in favor of a “voluntary” response (looking at a position where no stimulus is present) on the basis of an instruction held in the working memory is per se a typical prefrontal function Frith et al 1991, Goldman-Rakic 1987, Roberts et al 1994, subsumed under the neuropsychological concept of “executive functions” Denckla 1996, Pennington and Ozonoff 1996.

So far, the CNV in schizophrenia has been investigated only using tasks that involve manual motor responses, with amplitude reductions being a frequently reported result (e.g., Abraham and McCallum 1976, Cohen 1989, Timsit-Berthier et al 1984, van den Bosch 1983). CNV amplitudes may normalize with remission from acute stages of the disease Knott et al 1976, McCallum and Abraham 1973 or with neuroleptic treatment Knott et al 1976, Tecce and Cole 1976. Indeed, neuroleptic treatment has been reported to increase the frontal brain metabolism in schizophrenia (e.g., Berman et al 1986, Buchsbaum et al 1987). The CNV amplitude reduction may be considered part of the frontal hypometabolism that has been supported by regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) studies during the execution of tasks sensitive to frontal dysfunctions Andreasen et al 1992, Frith et al 1991, Lewis et al 1992, Paulman et al 1990, Weinberger et al 1986.

Impaired performance during antisaccade tasks (e.g., Crawford et al 1998, Sereno and Holzman 1995) seems to be another consequence of the frontal dysfunction in schizophrenia. Despite normal latencies of prosaccades, schizophrenic patients need significantly more time than healthy subjects to generate correct antisaccades Danckert et al 1998, Fukushima et al 1990, Karoumi et al 1998, McDowell and Clementz 1997. Schizophrenic patients also produce more erroneous prosaccades during the antisaccade task than control subjects but are apparently able to correct all or at least most of them Clementz et al 1994, Fukushima et al 1988, Fukushima et al 1990, Karoumi et al 1998, McDowell and Clementz 1997. There is some direct evidence that links the antisaccade deficit of schizophrenic patients with dysfunctions of the frontal lobes. First, the deficit is more frequently observed in schizophrenic patients with abnormal frontal computed tomography (CT) scans Fukushima et al 1988, Fukushima et al 1990. Second, during a task similar to the antisaccade task, the generation of “volitional” saccades was associated with an increase in FEF and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) metabolism in healthy participants but not in schizophrenic patients (Nakashima et al 1994). Finally, in schizophrenic patients the antisaccade task performance covaries with performance in other tasks sensitive to frontal dysfunctions, such as the smooth pursuit eye movement task Schlenker and Cohen 1995, Sereno and Holzman 1995 or the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Karoumi et al 1998, Nkam et al 1998, Rosse et al 1993; nonsignificant correlations with WCST were reported by Schlenker and Cohen [1995]).

The aim of our study is the investigation of the CNV preceding pro- and antisaccades in schizophrenic patients as compared to healthy control subjects. This study expects the following results: 1) A slow surface-negative potential shift, the saccadic CNV, with greater amplitudes preceding anti- as compared to prosaccades, should arise in healthy control subjects and patients; 2) schizophrenic patients should exhibit reduced CNV amplitudes during all saccade tasks, and a significantly smaller CNV amplitude modulation than control subjects when preceding antisaccades as compared to prosaccades; and 3) normal prosaccadic but augmented antisaccadic latencies along with augmented rates of erroneous prosaccades during the antisaccade task should be found in schizophrenic patients.

Section snippets

Participants

Data from 35 of a total of 39 subjects who participated in the experiment were analyzed. Data of three schizophrenic patients and one control participant had to be excluded because of artifacts (eye movement and other movement artifacts). The remaining sample comprised 17 patients treated for schizophrenic disorders (12 men, 5 women; mean age 29.9 ± 7.9 years; mean education = 10.3 ± 2.4 years; mean age of onset = 24.8 ± 6.4 years; mean number of episodes = 3.0 ± 2.6). All patients received an

Saccadic eye movements

Whereas prosaccade latencies were similar in schizophrenic patients (238.8 ± 34.6 msec, range 188–317 msec) and in healthy control subjects (237.2 ± 26.1 msec, range 202–281 msec), antisaccade latencies were significantly longer in patients (288.8 ± 66.5 msec, range 187–444 msec) compared to control subjects [259.1 ± 48.4 msec, range 188–379 msec; CONDITION: F(1,33) = 28.8, p < .001; CONDITION × GROUP: F(1,33) = 4.4, p < .05; GROUP: F < 1.5]. Furthermore, schizophrenic patients made more

Overview

The present study yielded the following main results: 1) Antisaccades were significantly slower than prosaccades in both groups; 2) in comparison to healthy control subjects, schizophrenic patients exhibited delayed responding only during the anti-, but not during the prosaccade task. During the antisaccade task, patients committed more erroneous prosaccades than control subjects; 3) a centrally predominant saccadic CNV was found in both groups and during both tasks; 4) control subjects’

Acknowledgements

Research was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG; Kl 985/6-1).

The authors are grateful to two anonymous reviewers as well as Rolf Verleger and Rudolf Cohen for helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

References (94)

  • B Fischer et al.

    The analysis of saccadic eye movements from gap and overlap paradigms

    Brain Res Protocols

    (1997)
  • J Fukushima et al.

    Disturbances of voluntary control of saccadic eye movements in schizophrenic patients

    Biol Psychiatry

    (1988)
  • J Fukushima et al.

    Further analysis of the control of voluntary saccadic eye movements in schizophrenic patients

    Biol Psychiatry

    (1990)
  • J Fuster

    Behavioural electrophysiology of the prefrontal cortex

    Trends Neurosci

    (1984)
  • C Gómez et al.

    Response latencies and event-related potentials during the gap paradigm using saccadic responses in human subjects

    Int J Psychophysiol

    (1996)
  • P.E Hallett

    Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions

    Vision Res

    (1978)
  • P.E Hallett et al.

    The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel voluntary oculomotor task

    Vision Res

    (1980)
  • T Hamano et al.

    The cortical generators of the contingent negative variation in humansA study with subdural electrodes

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1997)
  • A Ikeda et al.

    Subdural potentials at orbitofrontal and mesial prefrontal areas accompanying anticipation and decision making in humansA comparison with bereitschaftspotential

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1996)
  • A Ikeda et al.

    Dissociation between contingent negative variation (CNV) and Bereitschaftspotential in patients with Parkinsonism

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1997)
  • A Ikeda et al.

    Dissociation between contingent negative variation and Bereitschaftspotential in a patient with cerebellar efferent lesion

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1994)
  • C Klein et al.

    Determinants of the post-imperative negative variation in schizophrenic patients and controls

    Schizophr Res

    (1996)
  • W Klostermann et al.

    The presaccadic cortical negativity prior to self-paced saccades with and without visual guidance

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1994)
  • M Lamarche et al.

    Intracerebral recordings of slow potentials in a contingent negative variation paradigmAn exploration in epileptic patients

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1995)
  • G McCarthy et al.

    Scalp distributions of event-related potentialsAn ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1985)
  • J.M Morgan et al.

    Frontocentral DC-potential shifts predicting behavior with and without a motor task

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1992)
  • Y Nakashima et al.

    Cortical control of saccade in normal and schizophrenic subjectsA PET study using task-evoked rCBF paradigm

    Schizophr Res

    (1994)
  • I Nkam et al.

    Saccadic eye movements and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia

    Schizophr Res

    (1998)
  • R.G Paulman et al.

    Hypofrontality and cognitive impairment in schizophreniaDynamic single-photon tomography and neuropsychological assessment of schizophrenic brain function

    Biol Psychiatry

    (1990)
  • F Perrin et al.

    Spherical spline for scalp potential and current density mapping

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1989)
  • I Rektor et al.

    Intracerebral recording of movement related readiness potentialsAn exploration in epileptic patients

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1994)
  • B Rockstroh et al.

    Event-related and motor responses to probes in a forewarned reaction time task in schizophrenic patients

    Schizophr Res

    (1994)
  • A.B Sereno et al.

    Antisaccades and smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia

    Biol Psychiatry

    (1995)
  • R Walker et al.

    Saccadic eye movements and working memory deficits following damage to human prefrontal cortex

    Neuropsychologia

    (1998)
  • T.C Weerts et al.

    The effects of eye fixation and stimulus and response location on the contingent negative variation (CNV)

    Biol Psychol

    (1973)
  • N Amador et al.

    Supplementary eye field activity during monkey performance of antisaccadic tasks

    Soc Neurosci Abstr

    (1995)
  • T.J Anderson et al.

    Cortical control of saccades and fixation in manA PET study

    Brain

    (1994)
  • N.C Andreasen et al.

    Hypofrontality in neuroleptic-naive patients and in patients with chronic schizophrenia

    Arch Gen Psychiatry

    (1992)
  • B Andresen et al.

    Positive, and Negative, and Disorganized Syndrome Scales for Schizophrenia (PANADSS)/Manual

    (1999)
  • P Berg et al.

    A multiple source approach to the correction of eye artifacts

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1994)
  • K.F Berman et al.

    Physiological dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. II. Role of neuroleptic treatment, attention, and mental effort

    Arch Gen Psychiatry

    (1986)
  • M.S Buchsbaum

    The frontal lobes, basal ganglia, and temporal lobes as sites for schizophrenia

    Schizophr Bull

    (1990)
  • B.A Clementz et al.

    Saccadic system functioning among schizophrenia patients and their first-degree biological relatives

    J Abnorm Psychol

    (1994)
  • R Cohen

    Event-related potentials and cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia

  • T.J Crawford et al.

    Saccadic eye movements in families multiply affected with schizophreniaThe Maudsley Family Study

    Am J Psychiatry

    (1998)
  • M.B Denckla

    A theory and model of executive functionA neuropsychological perspective

  • F Doricchi et al.

    Neural control of fast-regular saccades and antisaccades, an investigation using positron emission tomography

    Exp Brain Res

    (1997)
  • Cited by (75)

    • Emotional faces modulate eye movement control on an antisaccade task

      2020, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      Furthermore, some antisaccade studies have obtained cue-locked ERP waveforms, which could reflect cognitive processing related to response inhibition. In this regard, some studies have observed a Contingent Negative Variation (CNV) with higher amplitude prior to antisaccades than prosaccades (Ansari and Derakshan, 2011; Klein et al., 2000; Richards, 2003; Vanlessen et al., 2015). This may imply higher neural preparation resources to inhibit the reflex response and the re-orientation to the opposite side.

    • Happy and less inhibited? Effects of positive mood on inhibitory control during an antisaccade task revealed using topographic evoked potential mapping

      2015, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Preceding target onset, we focused on the CNV. We hypothesized that antisaccades would elicit a larger CNV than prosaccades, and that preparation in the positive mood group would be less efficient, specifically for antisaccades (in line with Ansari & Derakshan, 2011; Klein et al., 2000). Following target onset, we assessed whether positive mood would influence the N2, P3a and P3b, in such a way that these components would be expressed less strongly in the positive mood group, in addition to a modulation by saccade type.

    • Influence of positive subliminal and supraliminal affective cues on goal pursuit in schizophrenia

      2015, Schizophrenia Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      As a matter of fact, the fronto-central CNV and N2 are components mainly attributed to the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (van Veen and Carter, 2002; Gómez et al., 2003). Patients with schizophrenia have an altered ACC activation (Carter et al., 2001) and attenuations of the CNV (Klein et al., 2000) and N2 (van Veen and Carter, 2002) amplitude in cognitive tasks. This might suggest that the influence of affective pictures is all the more altered that the targeted cognitive processes are themselves altered.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text