Elsevier

Behaviour Research and Therapy

Volume 38, Issue 9, 1 September 2000, Pages 899-907
Behaviour Research and Therapy

Attempting suppression of traumatic memories over extended periods in acute stress disorder

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00120-5Get rights and content

Abstract

This study investigated the influence of attempted suppression and thought control strategies on traumatic memories. Survivors of civilian trauma with acute stress disorder (ASD; n=20) and without ASD (n=20) monitored their trauma-related thoughts for three 24-h periods. In period 1, participants were instructed to think about anything. In period 2, participants were administered suppression or nonsuppression instructions relating to thoughts of the trauma. In period 3, participants were again instructed to think about anything. The results revealed no evidence for an increase in trauma-related thoughts following suppression instructions. Punishment and worry thought control strategies correlated significantly with both anxiety and suppression ratings. Frequency of intrusions was associated with a distraction cognitive strategy. These findings point to the importance of traumatised individuals' cognitive strategies in mediating the management and occurrence of posttraumatic intrusions.

Introduction

The thought suppression paradigm provides one means of experimentally investigating the maintenance of psychopathological intrusions (Salkovskis, 1989). Consistent with this proposal, recent research has found that attempted suppression of naturally occurring negative intrusions results in an increase of the suppressed thoughts (McNally and Ricciardi, 1996, Salkovskis and Campbell, 1994, Trinder and Salkovskis, 1994). The role of attempted suppression in clinical conditions is questionable, however, because one study of spider phobics and controls found that whereas attempted suppression was associated with an increase in spider-related thoughts, the contribution of attempted suppression to the number of spider-related thoughts was relatively minimal (Muris, Merckelbach, Horselenberg, Sijsenaar & Leeuw, 1997).

One clinical domain that is characterised by intrusions is posttraumatic stress. In an initial study of thought suppression in acute stress disorder (ASD), Harvey and Bryant (1998) found that individuals with ASD reported an increase in traumatic memories following attempted suppression; this pattern was not evident in non-ASD subjects or ASD nonsuppressors. A similar result was found by Shipherd and Beck (1999) in their study of rape-related PTSD. These studies were limited, however, by their investigation of intrusive thoughts over periods of less than 10 min. These designs lack ecological validity because traumatised individuals typically avoid their memories over extended periods. In one study of attempted suppression over a four-day period, Trinder and Salkovskis (1994) found that suppressors reported more target thoughts and greater discomfort than subjects who only monitored their thoughts or actively thought about them.

The present study replicated and extended the study by Harvey and Bryant (1998) by requiring ASD and non-ASD subjects to either suppress or not suppress traumatic memories over 24-h, rather than 5-min periods. It also indexed the role of subjects' cognitive coping styles in attempted suppression. Previous findings have suggested that attempted suppression may play a subservient role to subjects' rehearsed avoidance strategies (Kelly & Kahn, 1994). On the basis that ASD subjects are characterised by worry and punishment strategies (Warda & Bryant, 1998), we predicted that these avoidant cognitive strategies would play a significant role in mediating attempted suppression.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were survivors of motor vehicle accidents (n=21), industrial accidents (n=6) or nonsexual assaults (n=13) who were referred to a large trauma hospital. Participants were excluded if they had sustained a traumatic brain injury, displayed poor English, or were using prescribed narcotic analgesia (with the exception of codeine) at the time of the study. Participants were allocated to the ASD group if they satisfied all DSM-IV criteria, and to the non-ASD group if they did not meet the

Participant characteristics

Table 1 presents the mean scores for participant characteristics. Separate 2 (diagnosis)×2 (suppression) analyses of variance (ANOVA) indicated no effect for age, and significant effects for diagnosis for trauma-assessment interval, F(1, 36)=11.00, p<0.01, length of hospitalisation, F(1, 36)=4.95, p<0.05, ASDI score, F(1, 36)=175.67, p<0.001, trauma severity score, F (1, 36)=7.76, p<0.01, IES-intrusion, F(1, 36)=53.20, p<0.001, IES-avoidance, F(1, 36)=106.09, p<0.001, BDI, F (1, 36)=27.47, p

Discussion

Whereas ASD participants reported employing more punishment and worry cognitive strategies to control their thoughts, non-ASD participants used more social control. This finding supports previous reports of the tendency for ASD participants to use punishment and worry for controlling trauma-related thoughts (Warda & Bryant, 1998), and is consistent with the prevalence of these strategies in other anxiety disorders (Amir et al., 1997, Wells and Davies, 1994). Use of punishment and worry

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the National Health and Medical Research Council. We are grateful to Michelle Moulds for assistance in data collection

References (18)

There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (48)

  • Cognitive-behavioral coping strategies associated with combat-related PTSD in treatment-seeking OEF-OIF Veterans

    2011, Psychiatry Research
    Citation Excerpt :

    Compared to OEF–OIF Veterans who did not screen positive for PTSD, those with a positive screen were more likely report using worry, self-punishment, social control, and behavioral distraction thought control strategies, and avoidance coping strategies to manage unwanted thoughts, feelings, and situations. This finding replicates and extends a growing body of research suggesting that strategies such as worry, self-punishment, and experiential avoidance are commonly employed to manage unwanted thoughts by individuals with PTSD (Roussis and Wells, 2008; Bennett et al., 2009; Scarpa et al., 2009), as well as several other psychiatric conditions (Amir et al., 1997; Warda and Bryant, 1998; Reynolds and Wells, 1999; Blalock and Joiner, 2000; Guthrie and Bryant, 2000; Morrison and Wells, 2000; Rassin et al., 2000; Rosenthal et al., 2006; Roussis and Wells, 2008; Barahmand, 2009). Consistent with cognitive therapy models (Wells, 2000), results of the current study suggest that OEF–OIF Veterans with PTSD are more likely than those without PTSD to engage in maladaptive cognitive-behavioral coping strategies that may decrease the cognitive flexibility needed for adaptive processing and recovery from trauma.

  • Sleep quality in treatment-seeking veterans of Operations Enduring Freedom and Iraqi Freedom: The role of cognitive coping strategies and unit cohesion

    2010, Journal of Psychosomatic Research
    Citation Excerpt :

    Maladaptive cognitive coping strategies, such as worry and fear of loss of vigilance, were associated with increased sleep difficulties. This finding replicates previous research suggesting that maladaptive cognitive coping strategies are more commonly employed by individuals with such psychiatric conditions as PTSD [34,35], acute stress disorder [36,37], generalized anxiety disorder [38], and major depression [38,39]. It extends these findings to suggest that worry and fear of loss of vigilance, which is related to a perceived loss of self-protection and the ability to respond optimally to threatening stimuli [16], are associated with sleep difficulties in OEF–OIF veterans.

  • Managing unwanted intrusive thoughts in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Relative effectiveness of suppression, focused distraction, and acceptance

    2009, Behaviour Research and Therapy
    Citation Excerpt :

    Moreover, when thoughts are associated with unpleasant emotions, people are likely to engage in chronic thought suppression (Wegner & Zanakos, 1994). The counterproductive effects of thought suppression have been observed in experimental studies of UITs (see review by Rassin, 2005), posttraumatic stress disorder (Shipherd & Beck, 1999), acute stress disorder (Guthrie & Bryant, 2000; Harvey & Bryant, 1998) and depression (Dalgleish & Yiend, 2006; Wenzlaff & Bates, 1998; Wenzlaff & Eisenberg, 2001; Wenzlaff, Meir, & Salas, 2002; Wenzlaff, Wegner, & Roper, 1988). These studies suggest that although suppression may seem to be an effective solution, it can have profound and unexpected consequences for the psychological influences of the unwanted thoughts.

  • Acute Stress Disorder Consequent to Motor Vehicle Collisions

    2008, Motor Vehicle Collisions: Medical, Psychosocial, and Legal Consequences
View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text