Elsevier

Behavior Therapy

Volume 32, Issue 3, Summer 2001, Pages 425-442
Behavior Therapy

Original Research
The relationship of child worry to cognitive biases: Threat interpretation and likelihood of event occurrence

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(01)80029-0Get rights and content

Worry, the cognitive component of anxiety, has been related to cognitive processing biases in adults. However, the relationship of child worry to cognitive biases has been largely unexplored. The present study examined this relationship. Worry in a community sample of 277 5th- and 6th-grade children was assessed with the Penn-State Worry Questionnaire for Children (Chorpita, Tracey, Brown, Collica, & Barlow, 1997). The Children's Opinions of Everyday Life Events (COELE) was designed to measure children's threat interpretation biases. Children's responses to worry-relevant vignettes were used to assess interpretation of ambiguous and threatening situations, degree of perceived threat, degree of situation-specific worry, and perceived likelihood of future occurrence. Results suggested strong relationships between child worry and threat interpretation biases. Compared to nonworriers, worriers interpreted both ambiguous and threatening situations as more threatening, expressed more worry in response to the events, and judged the events to have a higher probability of happening to them in the future. Temporal stability was only moderate, perhaps reflecting worry and cognitive biases as more variable phenomena in a community sample. Theoretical, clinical, and research implications of the findings are discussed.

References (50)

  • MattisonR.E. et al.

    Diagnostic utility of the Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale in children with DSM-III anxiety disorders

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (1988)
  • MeyerT.J. et al.

    Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire

    Behavior Research and Therapy

    (1990)
  • MurisP. et al.

    “Danger is lurking everywhere”: The relation between anxiety and threat perception abnormalities in normal children

    Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • PetersonL. et al.

    Gender and developmental patterns of affect, belief, and behavior in simulated injury events

    Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology

    (1997)
  • Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.)

    (1994)
  • AngelinoH. et al.

    Trends in the “fears and worries” of school children as related to socio-economic status and age

    Journal of Genetic Psychology

    (1956)
  • BarlowD.H.

    Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic

    (1988)
  • BarrettP.M. et al.

    Family enhancement of cognitive style in anxious and aggressive children

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (1996)
  • BauerD.

    An exploratory study of developmental changes in children's fears

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (1976)
  • BeidelD.C. et al.

    Comorbidity of test anxiety and other anxiety disorders in children

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (1988)
  • Bell-DolanD.J.

    Social cue interpretation of anxious children

    Journal of Clinical Child Psychology

    (1995)
  • Bell-DolanD.J. et al.

    Anxious children's social perceptions: Responses to provocation, peer group entry, and social failure situations

  • BorkovecT.D. et al.

    The nature of normal and pathological worry

  • ChorpitaB.F. et al.

    Cognitive processing in children: Relation to anxiety and family influences

    Journal of Clinical Child Psychology

    (1996)
  • CrickN.R. et al.

    A review and reformulation of social information-processing in children: Mechanisms in children's social adjustment

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1994)
  • Cited by (47)

    • Cognitive processes predict worry and anxiety under different stressful situations

      2022, Behaviour Research and Therapy
      Citation Excerpt :

      Interpretations can be generated at different stages of information processing: “online”, when individuals make immediate interpretations, to “offline”, when individuals generate them on reflection, thus requiring time to disambiguate the information (Hirsch, Meeten, Krahé, & Reeder, 2016). Initial studies (Krahé, Whyte, Bridge, Loizou, & Hirsch, 2019; Suarez & Bell-Dolan, 2001) show that negative offline interpretations are associated with greater trait worry. Feng et al. (2019) established that low worriers tend to interpret ambiguity in a benign manner (i.e., benign interpretation bias, including both positive and neutral interpretations) across both offline and online stages of processing when state worry was activated.

    • Using event-related potential and behavioural evidence to understand interpretation bias in relation to worry

      2019, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, understanding interpretation bias in adults with high levels of worry remains relatively unexplored since Suarez and Bell-Dolan (2001) used a sample of children and Krahé et al. (2019) used individuals with GAD in their sample. Although Suarez and Bell-Dolan (2001) compared groups of high and low worriers, they did not distinguish between the presence of negative interpretation bias and an absence of benign interpretation bias, which is important when developing strategies to lessen worry. If individuals have a negative interpretation bias, then reducing this negative bias may benefit individuals; while facilitating a benign bias may be more beneficial to individuals when they lack a benign interpretation bias.

    • Does reconsolidation occur in natural settings? Memory reconsolidation and anxiety disorders

      2017, Clinical Psychology Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      Several reports have shown that different cognitive-processing errors/distortions, i.e., dichotomous thinking, overgeneralization, selective abstraction, etc. (Beck, 1979; Clark & Beck, 2011; Knapp & Beck, 2008) facilitate anxiety elaboration and maintenance. GAD patients were found to generate more imperative (“have to/should”) and catastrophic words (i.e. “death”, “pain”) than control individuals (Beck & Dozois, 2011; Suarez & Bell-Dolan, 2001). High anxiety individuals also show an “emotional reasoning” bias, in which the state of anxiety experienced is interpreted as evidence of imminent threat (“If I feel anxious, there must be danger”; Arntz, Rauner, & van den Hout, 1995; Barlow, 2004; Beck, 1979).

    • Do beliefs about the utility of worry facilitate worry?

      2011, Journal of Anxiety Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      But negative beliefs could play an initial inhibitory role although later be counterproductive. From an information-processing point of view, pathological worry would be characterized by attentional and interpretative bias linked to threat schemata (Bradley, Mogg, Millar, & White, 1995; Butler & Mathews, 1987; Eysenck & Derakshan, 1997; Prados, 2008; Pratt et al., 1997; Provencher, Freeston, Dugas, & Ladouceur, 2000; Suarez & Bell-Dolan, 2001). And attention is also directed to worry itself too.

    • Worry and the anxiety disorders: A meta-analytic synthesis of specificity to GAD

      2010, Applied and Preventive Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Worry among youth also produces similar information processing biases as adults. For example, Suarez and Bell-Dolan (2001) found that among 5th- and 6th-grade children, worriers interpreted both ambiguous and threatening situations as more threatening, expressed more worry in response to the events, and judged these negative events to have a higher probability of occurring in the future compared to nonworriers. Furthermore, children and adults who worry at greater levels than others are more apt to detect potential threat (e.g., Taghavi, Dalgleish, Moradi, Neshat-Doost, & Yule, 2003), suggesting that worry may potentiate attention towards threat in the environment.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text