Sex differences in implicit punishment sensitivity: Evidence from two cognitive paradigms

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.017Get rights and content

Abstract

Men, relative to women, are more likely to engage in disinhibited behaviors – such as excessive alcohol consumption or violent crimes – that implicate lower levels of punishment sensitivity. The goal of the present two studies (N = 207) was to implicitly model punishment sensitivity in terms consistent with a process-based view of this construct. Study 1 found that women slowed down following error feedback in a cognitive task to a greater extent than men. Study 2 found that women, but not men, altered their predictions following error feedback in a purported precognition task. Results are discussed in relation to theories of sex differences, punishment sensitivity processes, and proneness to disinhibited behaviors.

Introduction

Sex differences in cognition and behavior were largely ignored prior to a classic review by Maccoby and Jacklin (1974). Since then, an increased focus on sex differences has occurred. It has since become apparent that one of the most prominent sex differences involves men’s greater tendency to engage in disinhibited behaviors of multiple types. Men commit 90% of homicides (Daly & Wilson, 1988) and commit the vast majority of other serious crimes as well (Bennett, Farrington, & Huesmann, 2004). Men are more likely to use and abuse alcohol and other drugs of a psychoactive nature (Walitzer & Dearing, 2006). Moreover, men desire and seek to instantiate sexual relations with barely known partners to a much greater extent than women (Buss & Schmitt, 1993). What appears to emerge from multiple literatures, then, is a robust sex difference in proneness toward disinhibited behavior, which appears more characteristic of men.

Gray, 1987, Gray, 1990 theory of disinhibited behavior has been especially influential to the personality and clinical literatures (e.g., Smillie, Pickering, & Jackson, 2006). Gray suggested that disinhibited behavior primarily, though not exclusively, involves low levels of punishment sensitivity – that is, a relative insensitivity to feedback indicating that behavioral tendencies are problematic (Fowles, 2006). Although this model has typically emphasized learning processes, it is also clear that reactivity to punishment feedback, in more proximal terms, is relevant to the theory (Patterson & Newman, 1993). Indeed, there are multiple sources of support for the idea that disinhibited individuals are less likely to alter their behaviors following punishment signals from the environment (Blair, 2004), though such results typically involve clinical populations.

From a self-regulation perspective, sensitivity to punishment signals is crucial. Self-regulation is conceptualized in terms of over-riding behavioral tendencies that are problematic and error-prone (Baumeister, Heatherton, & Tice, 1994). To the extent that individuals are punishment-insensitive, then, they are likely to repeat behaviors that have been punished in the past (Patterson & Newman, 1993). In more proximal terms, such individuals are likely to be non-responsive to error feedback and to fail to recruit regions of the frontal lobes responsible for mitigating problematic processing routines (Holroyd & Coles, 2002). For example, Kerns et al. (2004) found that individuals whose brains were more sensitive to error-proneness in a cognitive task were more capable of self-regulating problematic outcomes on the next trial. In sum, sensitivity to punishment feedback can be viewed as somewhat critical for modifying behavioral tendencies that are error-prone and disinhibited (Barkley, 1997).

Men’s greater proneness to disinhibited behavior of multiple types (reviewed above) might be understood in terms of their lesser tendencies to adjust behavior following punishment feedback. For example, problematic alcohol use engenders negative consequences related to lower levels of achievement, hangovers, missed meetings, and so on. The fact that men are more prone to binge drinking, alcohol dependence, and indeed to alcoholism (Walitzer and Dearing, 2006, Wilsnack et al., 2002) can therefore be plausibly interpreted as suggestive that heavy-drinking men (relative to women) continue their drinking habits despite evidence of their problematic nature.

Following Gray, 1987, Gray, 1990 influential theory, at least two prominent self-report measures of punishment sensitivity have been developed (Carver and White, 1994, Torrubia et al., 2001). It is now apparent that women score higher than men on such scales (Jorm et al., 1999, Li et al., 2007). However, Gray’s (1987) theory is a processing theory that would benefit from process-based assessments rather than self-reported ones. This is particularly true because reactivity to punishment feedback has been linked to brain structures such as the amygdala that mediate unconscious threat registration processes, which would not be introspectively available to the individual self-reporting such tendencies (e.g., LeDoux, 1996).

We therefore assessed potential sex differences in punishment sensitivity in implicit cognitive terms. In Study 1, we modeled such processes in terms of tendencies to slow down following error feedback. Although tendencies to slow down following error feedback have long been observed in normative terms (Rabbitt, 1968), there are preciously few studies linking individual differences in such tendencies to personality variables posited to involve lesser levels of punishment sensitivity (for a recent exception, see Wilkowski & Robinson, 2008).

A different implicit assessment paradigm was used in Study 2. A recent development in the decision-making literature suggests that punishment sensitivity processes can be modeled in terms of altering choices that have been previously punished (Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1997). In Study 2, we created a paradigm of this type in which tendencies to alter behavioral predictions following error feedback were measured. Studies 1 and 2 therefore cognitively model punishment sensitivity in terms of both RT (Study 1) and choice behaviors (Study 2). Convergence of results across these very different paradigms would therefore be particularly impressive.

We suggest that the implicit cognitive paradigms developed here may have general value to the personality-processing literature. Of primary importance, though, was to determine whether there is a sex difference in implicit punishment sensitivity that could perhaps account for men’s molar tendencies toward disinhibited behavior. If so, men should slow down following error feedback to a lesser extent than women in the Study 1 task. Further, men should be less likely to alter behavioral predictions following error feedback in the Study 2 task.

Section snippets

Study 1

Participants in Study 1 performed a choice reaction time task. When they made an incorrect response, error feedback was administered. Implicit punishment sensitivity was quantified in terms of tendencies to slow down following error feedback (relative to non-feedback) delivered on the previous trial. We hypothesized that women, relative to men, would exhibit this error-feedback pattern to a greater extent.

Study 2

Damage to certain key brain structures impairs the individual’s ability to learn from punishment feedback within tasks such as the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) (Bechara et al., 1997). However, the IGT may be problematic in other ways as it confounds implicit punishment sensitivity with adaptive responding (Dunn, Dalgleish, & Lawrence, 2006). To disentangle such influences, we designed a behavioral choice task in which the tendency to switch behavioral predictions following error feedback could

General discussion

We hypothesized a sex difference in implicit punishment sensitivity and provided support for such a processing difference in two studies. In Study 1, it was found that error feedback on one trial slowed responses to the next to a greater extent among women than men. In Study 2, it was found that the tendency to switch behavioral predictions following error feedback was much more characteristic of (and indeed exclusive to) women. Women, then, can be characterized in terms of higher implicit

References (49)

  • R.A. Barkley

    Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1997)
  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    Losing control: How and why people fail at self-regulation

    (1994)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy

    Science

    (1997)
  • S. Bennett et al.

    Explaining gender differences in crime and violence. The importance of social cognitive skills

    Aggression and Violent Behavior

    (2004)
  • D.M. Buss et al.

    Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating

    Psychological Review

    (1993)
  • J.P. Byrnes et al.

    Gender differences in risk-taking: A meta-analysis

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1999)
  • C.S. Carver et al.

    Action, emotion, and personality: Emerging conceptual integration

    Psychology Bulletin

    (2000)
  • C.S. Carver et al.

    Behavior inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1994)
  • M. Daly et al.

    Homicide

    (1988)
  • A.H. Eagly et al.

    The origins of sex differences in human behavior

    American Psychologist

    (1999)
  • D.C. Fowles

    Jeffrey Gray’s contributions to theories of anxiety, personality, and psychopathology

  • W.J. Gehring et al.

    Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • J.A. Gray

    The psychology of fear and stress

    (1987)
  • J.A. Gray

    Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition

    Cognition and Emotion

    (1990)
  • Cited by (10)

    • Sex differences in threat sensitivity: Evidence from two experimental paradigms

      2021, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Threat sensitivity, then, may be adaptive in supporting quicker avoidance actions (LeDoux, 1996), which can promote safety (Cross et al., 2011), yet mechanisms of this type also generate hedonic costs related to experiences of fear and anxiety (Perkins et al., 2020). Similarly, greater reactivity to negative feedback, which also seems to be more characteristic of women than men (Ding et al., 2017; Moeller & Robinson, 2010), can motivate corrective actions that mitigate future error (Botvinick, 2007), but can also result in disorganized behavior in the short-term (Klein & Robinson, 2019) as well as more intense experiences of anxiety, fear, and worry (Olvet & Hajcak, 2008). Whether threat sensitivity gives rise to beneficial or problematic consequences, that is, is likely to depend on a variety of factors related to outcome type, context, and time course (Olvet & Hajcak, 2008).

    • Self-adapting the success rate when practicing math

      2016, Learning and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      The second aim of Study 1 is therefore to relate preferences for success rates to a set of background variables that have demonstrated relations to individual differences in the tolerance for negative feedback: gender, ability, and age. Concerning gender, it is observed that females are more sensitive to punishment (Cross, Copping, & Campbell, 2011; Moeller & Robinson, 2010), pay more attention to negative stimuli (Li, Yuan, & Lin, 2008; Schirmer, Zysset, Kotz, & von Cramon, 2004; Yuan et al., 2009), and tend to avoid negative feedback to a higher extent (Weller, Levin, & Bechara, 2009), compared to males. It has been suggested that females are more concerned with pleasing adults, which might make them more anxious for encountering failures (Pomerantz, Altermatt, & Saxon, 2002).

    • Sex difference in the relationship between salivary testosterone and inter-temporal choice

      2015, Hormones and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      We believe this is an appropriate starting point as the vast majority of previous studies on DD measured response to gain (Kirby and Petry, 2004; Madden et al., 2009; Lucas and Koff, 2010; Shibata, 2013), except for some notable exceptions (Takahashi et al., 2006; Estle et al., 2006). However, considering that some studies have reported sex differences in terms of response to loss and punishment (Moeller and Robinson, 2010; Santesso et al., 2011), this is an interesting topic for future research. Third, we collected only one saliva sample from each participant.

    • Anxiety and Gender Influence Reward-Related Processes in Children and Adolescents

      2016, Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text