Elsevier

Early Human Development

Volume 35, Issue 3, 31 December 1993, Pages 193-205
Early Human Development

Differences and variations in the patterns of early independent walking

https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-3782(93)90106-5Get rights and content

Abstract

The early development of walking was investigated in 25 normal fullterm and in 25 low-risk preterm infants. All subjects were videorecorded within 3–4 weeks of the beginning of independent walking and again 4 months later. Analysis of the videos was carried out according to a predetermined list of items, with a semiquantitive score for each. The results indicate wide interindividual differences in normal infants in the form of independent walking. Several items seemed to be associated in different ways in different individuals. Age of onset, as long as the corrected age was considered, did not differ between preterm and fullterm infants, neither did their walking patterns. Gait asymmetries were often observed in both groups and they were related with asymmetries observable in prewalking locomotor behaviour. Toe-strike often occurred at the beginning of walking, but not after 4 months; in preterm infants the toe strike pattern correlated significantly with certain motor characteristics observed during the first weeks of life.

References (27)

  • F. Ferrari et al.

    Qualitative changes of general movements in preterm infants with brain lesions

    Early Hum. Dev.

    (1990)
  • E. Bates et al.

    From First Words to Grammar

    (1988)
  • N. Bayley

    Comparison of mental and motor test scores for ages 1–15 months by sex, birth order, race, geographical location and education of parents

    Child Dev.

    (1965)
  • M. Bottos et al.

    Locomotor strategies preceding independent walking: prospective study of neurological and language development in 424 cases

    Dev. Med. Child Neurol.

    (1989)
  • C.N. Burnett et al.

    Development of gait in childhood

  • C.N. Burnett et al.

    Development of gait in childhood

    Part II. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.

    (1971)
  • G. Cioni et al.

    The fetus in the extrauterine environment

    Ital. J. Neurol. Sci.

    (1986)
  • J. Cohen

    A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales

    Educ. Psychol. Meas.

    (1960)
  • H. Forssberg

    Ontogeny of human locomotor control I. Infant stepping, supported locomotion and transition to independent locomotion

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1985)
  • A. Gesell et al.

    Developmental Diagnosis

    (1947)
  • R. Griffith

    The Ability of Babies

    (1954)
  • D. Gorga et al.

    Trends in neuromotor behaviour of preterm and fullterm infants in the first year of life: a preliminary report

    Dev. Med. Child Neurol.

    (1985)
  • C.B. Heriza

    Comparison of legs movements in preterm infants at term age with healthy fullterm infants

    Phys. Ther.

    (1988)
  • Cited by (38)

    • Early human motor development: From variation to the ability to vary and adapt

      2018, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Also the kinematic parameters indicate that increasing experience is associated with the selection of preferred patterns (Polk et al., 2008). An example is the increased selection of the heel-strike pattern - that characterizes adult gait - from the early repertoire of varied foot placements (Cioni et al., 1993). The processes of adaptive selection are accompanied by improved walking proficiency, i.e., a decrease in step width and increases in step length and walking velocity – changes that occur especially during the first three months of walking experience (Chang et al., 2006; Ledebt et al., 1995; Sutherland et al., 1988).

    • Toddler exposure to flame retardant chemicals: Magnitude, health concern and potential risk- or protective factors of exposure: Observational studies summarized in a systematic review

      2017, Chemosphere
      Citation Excerpt :

      Furthermore, toddlers are at an increased risk of exposure because of their specific behavior, such as often being close to the floor while crawling and learning to walk, and putting hands and objects in their mouths (mouthing behavior) (Environmental Protection Agency, 2011). This specific risk heightening behavior does not apply to the entire toddlerhood period, which lasts from 1 to 3 years, as after 24 months toddlers have reduced mouthing behavior (Groot et al., 1998) and are able to walk independently (Cioni et al., 1993; Jeng et al., 2008). Besides the role of age and age related behavior, other factors may increase or decrease exposure.

    • Quantifying intra-limb coordination in walking of healthy children aged three to six

      2016, Gait and Posture
      Citation Excerpt :

      The development lasts the whole period of childhood; regarding the walking experience, mature processes are classified into two stages: The first stage spans the first 5–6 months after a child starts to walk, leading to rapid changes in terms of kinematics and kinetics [1,2]. Hallemans [3] first reported that a more mature gait becomes evident after 4 months of independent walking and the “heel-to-toe” roll-over pattern was established [4].

    • Development of walking in preterm and term infants: Age of onset, qualitative features and sensitivity to resonance

      2008, Gait and Posture
      Citation Excerpt :

      Only two studies have assessed the qualitative aspects of early walking movement in preterm infants. One showed poorer quality of walking movement in preterm infants as featured by greater gait width and poorer manoeuvrability [24]; whereas the other revealed no difference [22]. The discrepancy in these findings may be due to variation in sample characteristics (e.g., difference in gestational age and birth weight), definition of age of onset (difference in distance of walking), and assessment tool (video recording vs. free-field observation).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text