Do children panic?

https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(88)90064-5Get rights and content

Abstract

Clinical surveys and epidemiological studies suggest that few if any children experience unexpected, uncued (spontaneous) panic attacks. Similarly, few if any children present with panic disorder. Based on emerging behavioral and emotion theory views of panic, several “requirements” seem necessary for an individual to develop panic disorder. First, the individual must experience the abrupt physiological sensations characteristic of panic. In addition, empirical evidence suggests that the individual must have the cognitive capacity to experience attributions of “losing control,” or “going crazy.” Evidence from the literature on childhood hyperventilation syndrome suggests that children may be experiencing panic attacks without the cognitive component. Data on developmental stages in children's conceptions of illness from a Piagetian viewpoint suggests that children may not have the cognitive capacity to develop panic disorder (as opposed to simple phobia). Implications of these ideas for research on childhood anxiety and panic are outlined.

References (49)

  • B.A. Thyer et al.

    Agoraphobia: A test of the separation anxiety hypothesis

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1985)
  • D.J. Anderson et al.

    A comparison of panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (1984)
  • J.C. Anderson et al.

    DSM III disorders in preadolescent children: Prevalence in a large sample from the general population

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1987)
  • D.H. Barlow

    Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic

    (1988)
  • Barlow D.H. & Craske M.G. (in press). The phenomenology of panic. In S. Rachman & J.D. Maser (Eds.), Panic:...
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective

    (1985)
  • R. Bibace et al.

    Children's conceptions of illness

  • J. Broughton

    Development of concepts of self, mind, reality, and knowledge

    New Directions for Child Development

    (1978)
  • P. Buglass et al.

    A study of agoraphobic housewives

    Psychological Medicine

    (1977)
  • R.R. Crowe et al.

    A family study of panic disorder

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1983)
  • W. Damon

    Social and Personality Development

  • R.M. Doctor

    Major results of a large-scale pretreatment survey of agoraphobics

  • R.N. Emde et al.

    Emotional expressions in infancy

    (1976)
  • H.J. Eysenck

    The biological basis of personality

    (1967)
  • Cited by (75)

    • Adolescent development and risk for the onset of social-emotional disorders: A review and conceptual model

      2019, Behaviour Research and Therapy
      Citation Excerpt :

      One of the intriguing distinguishing features of many mental disorders is their different associations with particular life stages. For example, panic disorder has long been seen as a disorder that is rarely seen in childhood and several early theories tried to explain this specificity (Nelles & Barlow, 1988). Yet the same consideration has been rare among other forms of psychopathology.

    • Pediatric anxiety disorders

      2013, Asian Journal of Psychiatry
    • Sudden gains as a long-term predictor of treatment improvement among children in community mental health organizations

      2013, Behaviour Research and Therapy
      Citation Excerpt :

      Perhaps young children do not have sufficient cognitive insight to detect weekly internalizing changes. Research has demonstrated that age is associated with increased understanding of mental illness (Fox, Buchanan-Barrow, & Barrett, 2010; Nelles & Barlow, 1988). In one study, younger children were less likely to be able to make an internal attribution of their anxiety-related physical symptoms than were older children (Muris, Mayer, Freher, Duncan, & van den Hout, 2010).

    • Listen to your heart beat and shiver! An experimental study of anxiety-related emotional reasoning in children

      2010, Journal of Anxiety Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Finally, the study relied on a sample of 9–13-year-old children. While there are indications that children of this age certainly understand anxiety-related physical symptoms (e.g., Mattis & Ollendick, 1997; Muris et al., 2007), there are also authors who have argued that pre-adolescent children still lack the cognitive skills to really display more advanced cognitive distortions such as emotional reasoning (see Nelles & Barlow, 1988). Note that this also nicely fits with the (clinical) observation that panic disorder (symptoms) become really manifest during adolescence (e.g., Ollendick et al., 2004).

    • Theory-of-mind, cognitive development, and children's interpretation of anxiety-related physical symptoms

      2007, Behaviour Research and Therapy
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, it remains unclear whether such theories can be generalized to younger children. Some authors have suggested that panic disorder is less common at a young age, because children lack the cognitive ability to make the catastrophic misinterpretations of physical symptoms associated with panic (e.g., Nelles & Barlow, 1988). Although such reasoning makes sense, it is also true that few studies have actually made an attempt to examine young children's understanding and interpretation of anxiety-related physical symptoms.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text