Review
Segmentation versus integration in visual motion processing

https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-2236(93)90179-PGet rights and content

Abstract

Reliable motion perception requires processes that integrate visual motion signals from neighbouring locations in the visual field, which should have the effect of smoothing out spatial variations in velocity. However, we also require motion processing to be very sensitive to local velocity differences, so that moving objects appear sharply distinct from their background and specific differential properties of optic flow associated with the observer's motion can be detected. Perceptual experiments give evidence both for integrative processes, which lead to spreading of perceived motion, and for differential processes, which lead to motion contrast and segmentation. Current and future experiments might allow tests of theoretical schemes that employ adaptive networks and/or multiple representations in order to reconcile the conflicting demands of integration and segmentation.

References (58)

  • B.K.P. Horn et al.

    Artif. Intell.

    (1981)
  • O.J. Braddick

    Vision Res.

    (1974)
  • D. Regan et al.

    Vision Res.

    (1978)
  • K.I. Beverley et al.

    Vision Res.

    (1979)
  • P. Tynan et al.

    Vision Res.

    (1975)
  • M. Nawrot et al.

    Vision Res.

    (1990)
  • E.C. Hildreth

    The Measurement of Visual Motion

    (1984)
  • A. Yuille et al.

    Nature

    (1988)
  • E.H. Adelson et al.

    Nature

    (1982)
  • B. Julesz

    Foundations of Cyclopean Perception

    (1971)
  • S. Ullman

    The Interpretation of Visual Motion

    (1979)
  • W. Reichardt et al.

    Naturwissenschaften

    (1988)
  • S. Uras et al.

    Biol. Cybern.

    (1988)
  • M.V. Srinivasan

    Biol. Cybern.

    (1990)
  • T. Poggio et al.
  • S.M. Anstis

    Vision Res.

    (1970)
  • B.J. Frost et al.

    Science

    (1983)
  • J. Allman et al.

    Perception

    (1985)
  • J. Allman et al.

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (1985)
  • W. Reichardt et al.

    Biol. Cybern.

    (1979)
  • T. Poggio et al.

    Naturwissenschaften

    (1981)
  • M. Egelhaaf

    Biol. Cybern.

    (1985)
  • H.C. Longuet-Higgins et al.
  • J.J. Koenderink

    Vision Res.

    (1986)
  • A. Blake et al.

    Visual Reconstruction

    (1987)
  • J.J. Koenderink et al.

    J. Opt. Soc. Am.

    (1976)
  • K. Nakayama
  • B.C.J. Rogers

    Nature

    (1988)
  • H. Saito

    J. Neurosci.

    (1986)
  • Cited by (205)

    • A neural index of inefficient evidence accumulation in dyslexia underlying slow perceptual decision making

      2021, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      Noise exclusion is therefore necessary to form a perceptual filter and isolate the signal dots to detect motion (Dakin et al., 2005). Both integration and segregation have been recognised as competing visual forces previously (Braddick, 1993; Watamaniuk et al., 2003) and have been proposed to have distinct neural mechanisms (McDonald et al., 2013). Thus, it may not be the integration of motion information that is dysfunctional in dyslexia but rather an inability to segregate noise from signal information (Conlon et al., 2013).

    • No effect of feedback, level of processing or stimulus presentation protocol on perceptual learning when easy and difficult trials are interleaved

      2020, Vision Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      By contrast, receptive fields in higher cortical areas such as V3, V4 and V5, are much larger, and pool the incoming sensory information from lower level receptive fields (Burr & Thompson, 2011; Gilbert et al., 2001) across features such as space, time, speed, direction of motion and spatial frequency (Burr & Thompson, 2011; Gilbert, Sigman, & Crist, 2001; Felleman & Van Essen, 1987; Furlan & Smith, 2016; Hubel & Wiesel, 1965; Mikami, Newsome, & Wurtz, 1986; Movshon, Thompson, & Tolhurst, 1978; Sillito, Cudeiro, & Jones, 2006; Zeki, 1974). Local and global tasks have been developed in order to selectively assess these levels of processing (Newsome et al., 1989; Braddick, 1993; Gilbert et al., 2001; Bex & Dakin, 2002; Ostwald, Lam, Li, & Kourtzi, 2008; Amano, Edwards, Badcock, & Nishida, 2009; Burr & Thompson, 2011; Nishida, 2011). It should be noted that formal definitions of tasks for “local” and “global” processing do not exist, and the distinction between the two is not always clear cut, and therefore we propose these working definitions of local and global processing tasks.

    • Sensory loss due to object formation

      2020, Vision Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      For motion, it is well accepted that some form of segmentation occurs before motion information is integrated within segmented objects. Clearly, it would make no sense to integrate motion information across two distinct objects because these different objects are free to move in different directions and different speeds (Braddick, 1993; Masson & Perrinet, 2012). In comparison to the motion literature, the importance of segmentation in scene analysis is encountered less often in studies on binocular vision.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text