Brief communication
Odor identification: The blind are better

https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(86)90402-6Get rights and content

Abstract

Twenty sighted and twenty blind adults, 19 to 66 years of age, participated in tests of olfactory sensitivity to n-butyl alcohol and of identification of 80 everyday odors. The blind had poorer absolute sensitivity, but outperformed the sighted at identification. Age proved an important factor in the comparison; more than half the variance in identification within each group was ascribable to an age-related decline.

References (13)

There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (78)

  • Searching for individual multi-sensory fingerprints and their links with adiposity – New insights from meta-analyses and empirical data

    2022, Food Quality and Preference
    Citation Excerpt :

    Consistently, these studies found increased orthonasal olfactory sensitivity among blind compared to sighted individuals (cf. Gagnon, Kupers, et al., 2014). Such sensory compensatory phenomena have been observed when measuring odour awareness (Beaulieu-Lefebvre, Schneider, Kupers, & Ptito, 2011), olfactory detection threshold (Beaulieu-Lefebvre et al., 2011; Cuevas et al., 2010), discrimination (Cuevas et al., 2010; Cuevas, Plaza, Rombaux, De Volder, & Renier, 2009), identification of daily life odours (Cuevas et al., 2009; Murphy & Cain, 1986; Wakefield, Homewood, & Taylor, 2004) and odour localisation (Manescu, Chouinard-Leclaire, Collignon, Lepore, & Frasnelli, 2021). Supporting these empirical findings, Kupers et al. (2011) showed that specific zones of the occipital cortex were recruited for orthonasal olfactory processes in individuals with congenital blindness.

  • No Olfactory Compensation in Food-related Hazard Detection Among Blind and Deaf Adults: A Psychophysical Approach

    2020, Neuroscience
    Citation Excerpt :

    Possibly, enhancement of olfactory function follows sensory loss only in some groups of sensory-deprived subjects (e.g., Beaulieu-Lefebvre et al., 2011; Çomoğlu et al., 2015; Cuevas et al., 2010), and not in all blind/deaf people. Further, there is no specific perceptual task in which the compensatory effects are consistently reported, and some studies report even lower olfactory function in deaf (Diekmann et al., 1994; Guducu et al., 2016) or blind individuals (Murphy and Cain, 1986; Sorokowska, 2016). Still – it is also possible that we did not observe olfactory compensation because food-related hazard detection is relevant for everyone (Stevenson, 2010).

  • Treatments for smell and taste disorders: A critical review

    2019, Handbook of Clinical Neurology
    Citation Excerpt :

    Under the assumption that blind persons may rely more on odors than sighted persons and that they may be more “trained,” numerous studies have compared olfactory test scores of blind persons to those of sighted individuals. Most have observed no differences between the blind and the sighted (e.g., Schaefer et al., 1979; Diekmann et al., 1994; Schwenn et al., 2002; Cuevas et al., 2011; Luers et al., 2014; Kaya and Binar, 2016; Sorokowska and Karwowski, 2017), although exceptions exist (Murphy and Cain, 1986; Rombaux et al., 2010; Beaulieu-Lefebvre et al., 2011; Comoglu et al., 2015) (for reviews, see Smith et al., 1993; Sorokowska and Karwowski, 2017). The impact of training is evident in a study that found trained panel members of a water department to outperform both blind and sighted subjects on a variety of olfactory tests, including the UPSIT, a detection threshold test, and a 16-item discrimination test (Smith et al., 1993).

  • Early-blind Individuals Show Impaired Performance in Wine Odor Categorization

    2018, Neuroscience
    Citation Excerpt :

    Indeed, in the case of a forced-choice paradigm, the consensus is that blindness does not affect the performance (Smith et al., 1993; Rosenbluth et al., 2000; Schwenn et al., 2002; Cuevas et al., 2010; Beaulieu-Lefebvre et al., 2011; Luers et al., 2014; Comoglu et al., 2015; Gagnon et al., 2015; Sorokowska, 2016). However, in the case of a free naming identification task, several studies suggest that blind individuals outperform the sighted (Murphy and Cain, 1986; Rosenbluth et al., 2000; Wakefield et al., 2004; Cuevas et al., 2010; Rombaux et al., 2010; Renier et al., 2013; Gagnon et al., 2015), while only one team did not find significant results (Sorokowska, 2016; Sorokowska and Karwowski, 2017). Some authors have suggested that the heightened performance of blind individuals in free odor identification may be explainable by a greater ability for blind individuals to generate words (Burton et al., 2002), rather than from genuine increased olfactory ability.

View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text