Factors determining choice of a comparison other

https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(69)90048-1Get rights and content

Abstract

Subjects were tested in groups of nine for the presence of the positively valued trait of intellectual flexibility (Pos conditions) or the negatively valued trait of intellectual rigidity (Neg conditions). The subjects were told the approximate range of the group's test scores (R conditions) or they were not told the range (NR conditions). After the tests were scored, all subjects were told that they ranked fifth in the group of nine and were given their own scores. In the first variation of the experiment, the subjects were then asked to indicate which other score in the group (according to rank) they would most like to see. The subjects were then asked to indicate a second choice. It was predicted that: (1) NR subjects would attempt to determine the range by first choosing the highest numerical score, and (2) among R subjects, those in the Pos condition would choose a higher score for their first choice, while those in the Neg condition would choose a lower score, both groups thus comparing in the positively valued direction, and (3) among R subjects, the most frequent choice in the positively valued direction would be of the most similar other. All predictions were supported.

In the second variation, the subjects were asked to indicate which other person in the group they would like as an interaction partner later in the hour. As in the first experiment, a second choice was also obtained. The strongest tendency was for subjects to choose the two most extreme others in the positively valued direction, although there was also a significant tendency to choose the two most similar others, as well as the best and worst others.

When choosing a referent person for comparison, an individual's first need is to determine the boundaries of the scale. Given the scale boundaries, he attempts to confirm similarity with those better off and then to confirm dissimilarity with those worse off. The necessity of interacting publicly with the referent person increases choice of the most “attractive” others.

References (6)

There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (85)

  • Observation of other's preferential treatment as a constructive catalyst for improved repatronize intentions

    2020, Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services
    Citation Excerpt :

    As a result, a bystander's higher goal-setting motivation may lead to higher repatronize intention toward the firm. It is proposed that the constructive inspiration route in response to witnessing another's preferential treatment (i.e., perceived attainability → goal-setting motivation → repatronize intention) is most likely promoted when the target of preferential treatment is similar to the bystander (Ganegoda and Bordia, 2019; Goethals and Darley, 1977; Wheeler et al., 1969). However, scholars have examined perceived similarity to the comparison target in various ways.

  • The effect of rankings on honesty in budget reporting

    2014, Accounting, Organizations and Society
    Citation Excerpt :

    Specifically, prior research finds that individuals compare themselves to others whose performance is better than their performance (Nosanchuk & Erickson, 1985; Wheeler, Koestner, & Driver, 1982; Wood, 1989). For example, several studies find that individuals tend to compare themselves to individuals who are ranked above them (Gruder, 1971; Wheeler, 1966; Wheeler et al., 1969). Prior research, in turn, finds that these upward comparisons frequently lead individuals to increase their effort, performance, or rank (Buunk, Kuyper, & van der Zee, 2005; Huguet, Dumas, Monteil, & Genestoux, 2001).

  • PSYCHOLOGICAL ADJUSTMENT TO IMMIGRATION: INDIVIDUAL AND FAMILY ASPECTS

    2021, Psychological Adjustment to Immigration: Individual and Family Aspects
  • Job satisfaction and co-worker pay in Canadian firms

    2020, Canadian Journal of Economics
View all citing articles on Scopus

Work on this investigation was supported in part by Public Health Service Predoctoral Fellowship 1-F1-MH-36, 286-01 held by Butzine; 1-F1-MH-35, 916-01 held by Gruder; and a National Defense Act Title 4 Fellowship held by Jones.

2

Now at New York University.

3

Now at the College of William and Mary.

4

Now at the University of Illinois at Chicago Circle.

5

For comments on earlier versions of this article, we are indebted to John Arrowood and Bibb Latané.

View full text