A cross-sequential analysis of developmental differences in speed of visual information processing

https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(85)90063-3Get rights and content

Abstract

Development of processing speed was examined in three backward masking studies. The first verified the central nature of backward masking for children aged 8 and 11 years and for adults. The second suggested that task requirements were equivalent for children similar to those in Study 1, and that age differences in performance were not attributable to nonprocessing variables. The main cross-sequential study estimated speed of processing in 80 children (approximately 6 years to 13 years) and young adults using an inspection time task. Target exposure duration was varied to establish the time required to achieve a high level of discriminative accuracy. Estimates of processing speed increased until about 11–13 years of age; beyond this, the trend was less obvious, and it is possible that inspection time asymptotes at around the onset of adolescence. Performance improvement after 1 year could not be explained as resulting from practice since improvement among controls over a period of 2 weeks was significantly less. Correlations between estimates of inspection time made up to 2 years apart found the measure to be reliable.

References (30)

  • C. Brand

    General intelligence and mental speed: Their relationship and development

  • M.T.H. Chi

    Age differences in the speed of processing: A critique

    Developmental Psychology

    (1977)
  • M.C. Ferreira

    Visual backward masking in children and adults

  • R.N. Haber

    The impending demise of the icon: A critique of the concept of iconic storage in visual information processing

    The Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (1983)
  • C.K. Hertzog et al.

    The effect of practice on age differences in central perceptual processing

    Journal of Gerontology

    (1976)
  • Cited by (49)

    • Effects of prior testing lasting a full year in NCANDA adolescents: Contributions from age, sex, socioeconomic status, ethnicity, site, family history of alcohol or drug abuse, and baseline performance

      2017, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      To estimate practice effects, we used a version of the twice-minus-once-tested method (Anderson et al., 2001; Salthouse, 2015) by comparing longitudinally-determined change over the 1-year test interval and change estimated from cross-sectionally-determined developmental trajectories. Some studies have shown that the greatest practice effect occurs between the first and second testing (Ibrahim et al., 2015; Waber et al., 2012) but diminishes at later repeated testing, observed in studies of pre-adolescence and adolescence development (Falleti et al., 2006; Thomas et al., 2016) and of healthy adults (e.g., Anderson et al., 2001; Ivnik et al., 2000; Nettelbeck and Wilson, 1985; Salthouse and Tucker-Drob, 2008). Whether developmental change ultimately overrides practice effects with later follow-up testing remains to be examined.

    • Intelligence and embodiment: A statistical mechanics approach

      2013, Neural Networks
      Citation Excerpt :

      These arguments could explain their extensive long-term memory abilities and their relatively poor performance in conventional tests of cognitive performance; this argument also brings to mind studies on the rare savant syndrome in humans (i.e., extraordinary memory abilities often in the context of intellectual impairment), which is often associated with autism (Hart et al., 2008; Heaton & Wallace, 2004). Finally, it is important to emphasize that the processing speed provides a necessary but insufficient condition for intelligence (Nettelbeck & Wilson, 1985). Furthermore, the learning speed cannot be used as a measure of intelligence (Pearce, 2008).

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This work was supported by ARGS Grant A281/15381 to the first author. Commputer programs were written by T. Walwyn. All research work was carried out entirely by the second author as part of the requirements for a doctoral thesis supervised by the first author. Contributions to writing were about equal.

    View full text