Introduction

Researchers interested in adolescent development have paid sustained attention to the role of peers and the quality of adolescents’ peer relationships. This is natural given that adolescents spend more time with age-mates than parents, both in school and in leisure activities (Larson and Richards 1991; Zeijl et al. 2000), peers are highly salient models for desirable (Fitzgerald et al. 2012) and undesirable behaviors (Fortuin et al. 2015), and peer groups provide contexts where more intimate friendships and first romantic experiences are established. Moreover, adolescence is a sensitive period for processing and handling social interactions (Blakemore and Mills 2014) and peers play a particularly influential role for emotional, social, academic, and behavioral development (Brown and Larson 2009; Steinberg and Monahan 2007; Steinberg and Morris 2001). Not surprisingly, problems in peer relationships are often linked to maladjustment and compromised well-being, both concurrently and over time. While there is little doubt that adolescents’ peer relationships are important for later development, little systematic information is available concerning the mechanisms that explain why peer experiences go “under the skin” and “into the mind”. This is unfortunate because this information is needed in order to be able to prevent potentially negative consequences of peer problems.

Like other forms of interpersonal difficulties such as divorce, workplace mobbing, or family conflict, peer problems can be a significant source of stress, broadly defined as “environmental demands [that] tax or exceed the adaptive capacity of an organism, resulting in psychological and biological changes that may place persons at risk for disease” (Cohen et al. 1995, p. 3). Peer problems originate in interactions between individuals and the peer group and are distinct from friendship problems, which occur in the dyadic relationship between two individuals. Talking to the friend and trying to come to a mutually satisfying solution are common strategies to handle friendship problems in adolescence whereas peer problems tend to be associated with anger, avoidance, and withdrawal (Seiffge-Krenke 2011). Thus, distinguishing between friendship and peer stress is necessary. The present review focuses on peer victimization, which constitutes not only a common problem among adolescents but also a central topic in research on peer-related precursor of development.

Peer victimization is defined as intentional and repeated infliction of harm on a person by one or more peers that are usually more powerful in some regard, with studies reporting prevalence rates of up to 45 % of adolescents having experienced peer victimization (e.g., Craig et al. 2009). Several reviews support the notion that victims of bullying fare worse in terms of emotional (Ttofi et al. 2011; Wu et al. 2015) and psychosocial adjustment (Hawker and Boulton 2000), are more likely to develop psychotic (van Dam et al. 2012), psychosomatic (Gini and Pozzoli 2009), and externalizing problems (Reijntjes et al. 2011), are at greater risk for suicidal ideation and behaviors (Holt et al. 2015; van Geel et al. 2014), and perform less well academically (Nakamoto and Schwartz 2010). Why is this the case? Which processes are at play?

The Current Study

The central aim of this review is to contribute to a more systematic understanding of why peer victimization is linked to problems in emotional, social, academic, and behavioral development by synthesizing studies that examined mediating pathways. Mediating pathways are understood in the statistical sense, that is, variables or processes that are thought to explain a part of the variance between peer victimization in adolescence and outcomes. Studied quite frequently, no attempt has yet been made to systematize these mediation studies although they may provide insight into why peer experiences “go under the skin” and “into the mind”, thus eventually inform interventions that target the negative sequelae of peer victimization.

Methods

Search Strategy

Two databases (Web of Knowledge, EBSCOhost) were searched during April and May 2016, adhering to the following strategy: the string “peer victimization” OR “bullying-victimization” OR bully* OR victim* was entered as search term in the first step. Results were subsequently narrowed down by searching within the obtained results for the following: Adjustment OR Maladjustment OR Psychological OR Internalizing OR Externalizing OR Outcomes. In the third step, results were further narrowed down by searching within the obtained results for the following: adolesc* OR teen* OR youth and in the fourth step, results were restricted by searching within results for the following: mediat* OR indirect. Quotation marks were used around terms consisting of more than one word. This search procedure was followed by a further restriction to journal articles written in English language. No limitations with respect to publication year were applied.

Next, title scans were performed through which evidently unrelated studies or studies based on animals or on clinical samples were excluded. Studies with ambiguous titles were retained at this stage. The title scan was followed by an abstract scan in which evidently unfitting studies were excluded. Again, studies with ambiguous abstracts were retained. The remaining studies were scanned in full-text and studies that did not examine peer victimization as independent variable, its relation to an outcome, as well as mediation of this proposed association, were excluded.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Studies were eligible for review if they focused on correlates of peer victimization and explicitly studied explanatory variables (mediators) or indirect effects through third variables. Adolescence was defined in accordance with the World Health Organization as ages 10–19 and only studies in which exposure was measured during this period were included in this review. Studies with several measurement occasions or where samples spanned many ages were included if at least one assessment was between the ages of 10 and 19 or where the mean age of the sample was reported and fell within this range. Only original empirical work was included, thus meta-analyses, annotations, comments, and reviews were excluded. Studies were also excluded if they focused on specific populations such as ethnic or sexual minorities or clinical samples, did not address the association between peer victimization and a correlate conceptualized as outcome of peer victimization, or did not study mediation of this link or indirect effects. In keeping with the statistical definition of mediation and requirements regarding formal testing, we also excluded qualitative studies from this review.

Results

Study Extraction

The flow diagram (Fig. 1) details the selection procedure. After removing duplicates that emerged in both databases, 141 potentially relevant results for peer victimization were obtained and were submitted to full-text study, which resulted in further exclusion of 76 articles on the following grounds: articles were reviews, did not study peer victimization as predictor or independent variable but instead only as mediator or dependent variable, participants were too young or too old at the time of peer victimization exposure, studies focused on more general concepts such as violent victimization or negative life events and included peer victimization in addition to various other risks into one single score, or no mediation or indirect effects were tested. The subsequent detailed synthesis is thus based on 65 articles.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow diagram depicting study selection. Note The search in Web of Knowledge was based on “all collections”, not “core collections” as is default since July 2016

Summary of Included Studies

Details about geographic region where the study was conducted, sample sizes, age group, measures of victimization, outcome, and mediator, as well as details on study and analytic design are presented in Table 1. The majority of the 65 studies that met the inclusion criteria were conducted in the United States (52 %) and other English-speaking contexts such as Canada (5), Australia (5), New Zealand (1), and the United Kingdom (4), as well as European countries (13), but there were also studies based on samples from India (Narayanan and Betts 2014) and Korea (Moon et al. 2012). Seventeen studies were based on short- and long-term longitudinal data, two studies used daily assessments over a two-week period, one study was conducted online only with participants from all over the world, one study was based on experimental data, and one study used functional neuroimaging. The remaining studies (68 %) were based on cross-sectional data.

Table 1 Studies (n = 65) on which the qualitative review is based

Measurements of peer victimization varied greatly but several studies used instruments developed by Crick and colleagues (Crick and Bigbee 1998; Crick and Grotpeter 1995; Cullerton-Sen and Crick 2005) and many authors relied on Olweus’ definition and measurement of bullying (Olweus 1996). With respect to outcomes, 19 studies focused solely on facets of internalizing problems such as depression, anxiety, and loneliness, six studies examined self-harm and suicide ideation as outcomes, two studies examined substance use, five studies solely examined externalizing problems, and four studies examined outcomes related to weight and eating behavior in relation to peer victimization. School-related outcomes such as academic achievement and school adjustment were examined in 14 studies. Eight studies included a combination of the outcomes listed above and seven studies focused solely or additionally on other outcomes including somatic complaints, post-traumatic symptoms, self-esteem, self-efficacy, and young people’s willingness to befriend another victim.

Studies differed in whether they reported decreasing sizes of direct effects between peer victimization and outcome under study or Sobel-z-statistics or comparable test statistics, with some studies reporting both. Some studies estimated nested path or structural equation models and compared changes in model fit in relation to loss or gain in parsimony. A number of recent studies made use of the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Hayes 2012), which estimates indirect effects and indicates their confidence intervals, thus allowing conclusions about the significance of mediation pathways.

In the following, studies are described as reporting mediation if the Sobel-z-statistic or other indirect effect measures are statistically significant or if systematic comparisons between model fit indices were conducted. If mediation is inferred solely from the decreasing size of the direct effect between peer victimization and outcome when controlling for the mediator (Baron and Kenny 1986) or from connecting two correlations with each other but not confirmed by calculating the significance of the indirect effect, this is pointed out explicitly.

Results of Individual Studies

Mediators on Associations Between Peer Victimization and Internalizing Outcomes

The subsequent section includes all studies that tested mediation of associations between peer victimization and internalizing problems, regardless of whether one type (usually depression or depressive symptoms) or a combination (usually depression and anxiety) of symptoms were examined. In a broad sense, emotion-related constructs, self-perceptions, and emotional and behavioral reactions to peer victimization were studied as potential explanations for internalizing problems in victimized adolescents. For instance, hopelessness was examined in two studies, one of which did not find a link between relationally oriented peer victimization and the mediator (Hamilton et al. 2013), thus did not test for mediation, whereas the second study (based on the same sample) showed that hopelessness mediated the link between peer victimization and depression but only among adolescents with low future orientation (Hamilton et al. 2015). Other emotion-related mediators included rejection sensitivity, which describes anxious expectations of not being accepted or of being overtly rejected. Victimized adolescents scored higher on rejection sensitivity, which, in turn, was linked to greater loneliness and more depressive symptoms (Zimmer-Gembeck et al. 2014). More broadly, emotion dysregulation, a construct comprising of poor emotional understanding, dysregulated expression of anger and sadness, and rumination, mediated the association between relational and reputational victimization and internalizing symptoms (McLaughlin et al. 2009).

Separating different internalizing symptoms, Baker and Bugay (2011) showed that self-reported loneliness mediated the effect of victimization in a model with depression as outcome. Hamilton et al. (2016) explored depression as mediator between peer victimization and anxiety and, vice versa, anxiety as mediator on the link between peer victimization and depression. Only the latter was empirically supported, that is, victimized adolescents were at greater risk for depression, which, in turn, increased their social anxiety risk. This model used longitudinal data and controlled for earlier levels of maladjustment, which lends support to the strength of the association.

Self-perceptions were examined frequently but empirical support for their role in explaining associations between peer victimization and internalizing problems is inconsistent. Some studies reported mediation by self-esteem and self-evaluations (Lopez and DuBois 2005; Raskauskas et al. 2015; Ybrandt and Armelius 2010) whereas others did not formally test for mediation because of lacking direct associations between self-esteem/self-concept and peer victimization (Bosacki et al. 2007; Turner et al. 2010). One study examined body esteem as a specific facet of self-esteem and found a mediating role among early maturing Caucasian and late maturing African American girls only (Hamlat et al. 2015).

Associations between peer victimization and internalizing problems were further mediated by cognitions and behaviors relatively proximal to peer victimization as such. For instance, Barchia and Bussey (2010) showed that victimized adolescents believed less strongly that that their school was able to counter violence, which also resulted in less help-seeking. On the other hand, victimized adolescents were more likely to ruminate. These pathways (rumination on the one hand and belief in collective efficacy and enlisting help on the other) explained in this study the association between victimization and depression. As shown by Dao et al. (2006), victims of bullying perceived a greater threat of future victimization, which explained their increased risk for psychological distress, but indirect effects were not explicitely calculated in this study. Self-blame was studied by Chen and Graham (2012) and Perren et al. (2013). Chen and Graham found support for the notion that victimized adolescents often blamed themselves for what had happened, which contributed to greater risk for maladjustment. In contrast, Perren and colleagues found no support for associations between victimization and self-blame, or self-blame and internalizing problems.

Finally, three studies examined mediators reflective of adolescents’ social relationships. In detail, Woodhouse et al. (2012) showed that social acceptance mediated the association between peer victimization and loneliness whereas associations between various other social behaviors and loneliness were not explained by social acceptance. The authors also reversed the model and explored whether victimization mediated the association between social acceptance and loneliness but no support was found for this model. Estevez et al. (2005) examined openness, avoidance, and offensive tone in communication with parents as well as teacher’s perception of the adolescent in terms of adjustment, integration at school, and with respect to the student–teacher relationship as mediators of peer victimization effects on psychological distress. Only adolescents’ communication with fathers explained this link. Somewhat puzzling, Herrero et al. (2006) tested almost the same model and based their study on the same data and as Estevez et al. (2005), but did not report or account for the previously reported indirect effect from peer victimization to psychological distress through father communication, thus it is not clear whether this mediation is stable to changes in the structure of the rest of the model.

Quite different from these mostly questionnaire based studies is a neuroimaging study by Casement et al. (2014), who showed differences between victimized and non-victimized girls in activation of the medial prefrontal cortex, which is implicated in memory and decision making. The authors had hypothesized that individual response variation in this area would be related to depression, thus potentially constituting a mediating pathway between peer victimization and internalizing problems, but explicit mediation tests did not support this pathway.

Mediators on Associations Between Peer Victimization and Self-Harm and Suicide Ideation

Four of the six studies that focused on self-harm and suicide ideation examined depressive symptoms and similar constructs (negative affect and alexithymia, which describes difficulty in identifying one’s own emotions) as mediators and all four studies reported significant effects (Claes et al. 2015; Garisch and Wilson 2010; Hay and Meldrum 2010; Lereya et al. 2013). The fifth study (Henry et al. 2014) examined meaning in life as mediator but found support only for females, whereas the sixth study (Litwiller and Brausch 2013) showed that substance use and violent behavior partly explained the association between traditional and cyber-victimization and suicidal behavior.

Mediators on Associations Between Peer Victimization and Externalizing Outcomes

Externalizing outcomes varied greatly. For instance, den Hamer et al. (2013), Moon et al. (2012), Povedano et al. (2015) examined whether victimized adolescents were more likely to become perpetrators of bullying, whereas Herts et al. (2012) examined aggression as correlate of peer victimization, and Hinduja and Patchin (2007) focussed on delinquency.

In more detail, Povedano et al. (2015) found that victimized adolescents were more lonely and less likey to comply to school norms, which explained their greater risk for violent behavior towards schoolmates, though indirect effects were not tested explicitely. Moon et al. (2012) examined whether anger explained why victims sometimes turn to bullying others but found no support for this mediator, which contrasts den Hamer et al. (2013) who did find that anger mediated the association between being a victim and being a bully. The latter study also examined another mediator—use of media with antisocial content-, arriving at a “cyclic process model” where anger and frustration were interpreted as immediate reactions to victimization that resulted in greater use of media with antisocial content, which, eventually, increased the risk to become a perpetrator of cyberbullying.

Turning to studies that explored problem behaviors as more general correlate of peer victimization, Herts et al. (2012) showed that victimized adolescents were less well able to regulate their emotions, which placed them at greater risk for aggressive behavior, whereas Hoglund and Leadbeater (2007) found that lower social perspectice awareness, that is, adolescent’s awareness of their peers’ thoughts and feelings, functioned as mediator between peer victimization and aggression.

Several studies (Harper 2012; Lopez and DuBois 2005; Perren et al. 2013; Singh and Bussey 2011; Ybrandt and Armelius 2010; You and Bellmore 2012) utilized Achenbach’s measures (Achenbach et al. 2003), specifically the Youth Self-Report, Child Behavior Checklist, and Teacher Report to assess associations between peer victimization and externalizing problems, though studies varied with respect to the mediator tested. For instance, Lopez and DuBois (2005) and Ybrandt and Armelius (2010) found that lower levels of self-esteem partly explained why victimized adolescents showed greater externalizing problems and Harper (2012) found that self-blame was to some extend responsible for victim’s higher risk for externalizing problems. The multi-outcome, multi-mediator study by Perren et al. (2013) revealed that victims were more likely to attribute the victimization event to hostile intentions in their peers. Such attributions, in turn, were linked to externalizing problems. Singh and Bussey (2011) showed that adolescents’ self-efficacy for avoiding aggressive behavior, that is, their ability to forgive and avoid revenge-seeking mediated the link between peer victimization and externalizing problems. Finally, You and Bellmore (2012) found that victimized adolescents experienced more conflict in their friendships, which partly explained their greater risk for externalizing problems.

Mediators on Associations Between Peer Victimization and Substance Use

Two studies were identified that examined substance use as correlate of peer victimization and both focused on mediation by drinking motives, that is, explored whether why victimized adolescents drink and whether these reasons affected frequency, quantity, and severity of their alcohol consumption. Whereas Topper et al. (2011) tested an overall composite of enhancement, social, conformity, and coping motives, Archimi and Kuntsche (2014) examined each of the motives separately. Irrespective of approach, both studies found evidence for mediation, particularly enhancement and social motives (Archimi and Kuntsche 2014).

Mediators on Associations Between Peer Victimization and Eating-Related Outcomes

Four studies linked peer victimization to eating behavior, suggesting that victimized youth might overeat (Mamun et al. 2013) or skip meals (Sampasa-Kanyinga et al. 2014), and aiming to elucidate underlying pathways for such associations. For girls, the association between verbal victimization and body dissatisfaction was mediated by emotional symptoms whereas this mechanisms was not observed for physical or social peer victimization or restrained eating as outcome (Farrow and Fox 2011). Similarly, psychological distress to some extend explained why verbally victimized girls skip their breakfast (Sampasa-Kanyinga and Willmore 2015) and depression mediated the association between cyber-victimization and breakfast-skipping in another study by the same group (Sampasa-Kanyinga et al. 2014). It remains unclear whether non-significant results for other forms of victimization (e.g., physical) are systematic or study-dependent.Footnote 1

Mediators on Associations Between Peer Victimization and School-Related Outcomes

Fourteen studies focused solely on school-related correlates of peer victimization, specifically on academic or school adjustment, achievement, school affection/connectedness, and school safety, and Lopez and DuBois (2005) included academic adjustment in their multi-outcome study. Various mediators were tested as explanations for links between peer victimization and academic achievement, including maladjustment, self-perceptions, classmate support, and sleep problems. With respect to maladjustment, Hoglund (2007) reported mediation by externalizing problems on the link between relational victimization for boys and physical victimization for girls. Totura et al. (2013) reported mediation by psychological distress. Moreover, sleep problems (Lepore and Kliewer 2013), lower levels of self-esteem and self-efficacy (Raskauskas et al. 2015; Thijs and Verkuyten 2008), and reduced classmate support (Wang et al. 2011) were found to explain why victimized adolescents fare less well with respect to academic achievement, though indirect effects via the latter three were not explicitly tested.

Turning to academic adjustment, the studies reviewed here suggest that effects of peer victimization are mediated by distress (Espinoza et al. 2012), psychological adjustment (Graham et al. 2006; Rueger and Jenkins 2014), daily emotions (Espinoza 2015), self-blame (Graham et al. 2006), and negative self-evaluation (Lopez and DuBois 2005).

Finally, conflicting results were reported with respect to maladjustment as mediator between peer victimization and school affection/connectedness—whereas Galand and Hospel (2013) showed that depression in combination with self-efficacy mediated this link, no support for associations between internalizing or externalizing problems and school connectedness was found by Loukas et al. (2012).

Mediators on Associations Between Peer Victimization and Other Outcomes

A small number of studies examined outcomes other than the ones reviewed above. In detail, Ma and Bellmore (2012) examined whether internalizing symptoms mediated associations between peer victimization and parental control but, different to a large body of literature on the topic, found no initial link between victimization and internalizing symptoms. Guzzo et al. (2014) showed that alexithymia mediated the association between victimization and post-traumatic symptoms and avoidant coping mediated the association between appearance-related victimization and self-esteem (Lodge and Feldman 2007). In contrast, Narayanan and Betts (2014) found no support for a mediational role of resilience in the association between victimization and self-efficacy. Finally, Herge et al. (2016) found support for mediating roles of depression and anxiety in the prediction of somatic and sleep symptoms and Guarneri-White et al. (2015) showed that general depression but not anxious depression or post-traumatic symptoms explained why victimized adolescents tend to experience more health problems like headaches, fatigue, and vomiting.

Discussion

A total of 65 studies on mediators of associations between peer victimization and a variety of outcomes including internalizing and externalizing problems, self-harm and suicide ideation, eating and substance use behaviors, and school-related outcomes were reviewed, most of which presenting cross-sectional data of American youth. Most frequently studied outcomes concerned internalizing symptoms, which is largely in line with the literature on peer victimization as such, and school-related outcomes including achievement and adjustment. In comparison, only a minority of studies examined mediation of externalizing behavior and substance use. Most commonly studied mediators concerned emotional states or symptoms, as well as self-perceptions and attributions.

In line with other reviews, direct associations between peer victimization and emotional, social, academic, and behavioral measures were found in most studies reviewed here but the large number of outcomes of peer victimization and mediators tested makes it difficult to succinctly summarize the pathways that explain why peer victimization in adolescence affects adjustment and well-being. There are a few exceptions to this, for instance concerning substance use, which was mediated by drinking motives in two different studies, as well as eating-related outcomes and self-harm and suicide ideation, which were relatively consistently mediated by emotional symptoms. Emotions and emotional symptoms also explained many of the associations between victimization and school-related outcomes and with respect to health problems.

A different pathway that was also tested in various studies concerned self-perceptions and attributions. Whereas negative self-evaluations and self-esteem mediated school adjustment and achievement (Graham et al. 2006; Lopez and DuBois 2005; Raskauskas et al. 2015) and externalizing problems (Ybrandt and Armelius 2010), findings were more mixed with respect to internalizing outcomes and might be dependent on other individual or contextual factors. Similarly, self-blame did not emerge as a stable mediator although tested repeatedly and with respect to different outcomes (Chen and Graham 2012; Graham et al. 2006; Harper 2012; Perren et al. 2013).

Taken together, these findings might suggest that victimized adolescents first and foremost experience change in emotions or develop emotional problems in reaction to peer victimization, which are subsequently expressed in other forms of maladjustment, such as restrained eating, self-harm, or school problems. Social-cognitive processes might be less salient than emotional processes and might be less relevant in explaining why peer victimization affects adjustment and wellbeing. The summary of the reviewed studies suggests that explanations might generally be quite complex because chain mediations are likely where, for instance, peer victimization increases loneliness (Baker and Bugay 2011), which results in more depressive symptoms, which, in turn, predict higher anxiety (Hamilton et al. 2016). Future studies are needed that elucidate the sequence of consequences of peer victimization.

With respect to methodology, the progression in analytic software is evident from analytic designs. Whereas some studies relied exclusively on the Baron and Kenny approach of mediation testing (1986) where mediation is inferred from comparing direct effect sizes with and without controlling for the mediating variable, studies increasingly used Hayes’ well-documented and freely available SPSS macro PROCESS (e.g., Casement et al. 2014; Guarneri-White et al. 2015; Hamlat et al. 2015; Mathieson et al. 2014). Such novel approaches are needed to estimate possible double- and multiple-mediation, as are larger sample sizes than used in some of the studies reviewed here (e.g., Baker and Bugay 2011; Espinoza 2015; Giannotta et al. 2012; Guarneri-White et al. 2015; Harper 2012; Jenkins and Demaray 2015, who all used samples smaller than n = 200).

In addition to small sample size in some of the studies reviewed here, other limitations need to be noted. For example, there is little geographic variation with complete absence of studies using South American or African samples. While a preponderance of North American and European samples is common in psychological research, this means that findings cannot be generalized to samples from other regions of the world.

Moreover, the majority of studies relied on cross-sectional samples. Although these are more easily acquired and thus likely allow for larger samples, tests of mediation typically require longitudinal assessments. Requiring participants to undergo assessments at two or more time points carries the risk of attrition but contemporary analytic procedures such as path models and multiple imputation can be used to lessen the impact of missing data.

Another limitation of this research is that the majority of studies relied on school samples where adolescents themselves completed questionnaires and did not collect reports from other sources (but see Boulton 2013; Graham et al. 2006; Ma and Bellmore 2012; Perren et al. 2013; Singh and Bussey 2011; Woodhouse et al. 2012 who used peer nominations for victimization). Together with cross-sectional assessments, this likely results in shared method variance, which raises doubt as to whether direct and indirect effects in the studies reviewed here are over-estimated. Well-powered longitudinal studies were not common among the studies reviewed here (but see Barchia and Bussey 2010; Herge et al. 2016; Herts et al. 2012; Lereya et al. 2013; Mamun et al. 2013; McLaughlin et al. 2009; Moon et al. 2012; Rueger and Jenkins 2014; Turner et al. 2010); however, such study designs are needed to identify replicable indirect effects of small size.

Finally, a number of limitations with respect to the selection and review procedure are noted. To begin with, there is always the possibility of incomplete retrieval though the search procedure was reviewed repeatedly throughout the process. Articles acquired from elsewhere were not included in this review to ensure objectivity and easy reproducibility of the review; this might have resulted in omitting other suited studies. Similarly, only English-speaking journal articles were included in this review, omitting the work done in other languages and studies published in dissertation or book format.

Conclusion

Aiming to systematically summarize research into explanations for why peer victimization increases adolescents’ risk for maladjustment and compromised well-being, 65 articles were selected and reviewed. Most studies examined internalizing and school-related outcomes and associations between peer victimization and outcomes were most stably mediated by emotions and emotional symptoms. The restricted geographic range, cross-sectional designs and single-reporter bias are important limitations that need to be tackled in future studies.