Skip to main content
Log in

Using Expressive Writing to Test an Evolutionary Hypothesis About Depressive Rumination: Sadness Coincides with Causal Analysis of a Personal Problem, Not Problem-solving Analysis

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Evolutionary Psychological Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The analytical rumination hypothesis proposes that depression is an evolved response to complicated life stressors or problems. As part of this response, depressive symptoms promote analytical rumination, a prolonged and distraction-resistant style of thinking that functions to address the triggering problem. Analytical rumination involves analytical thinking about why the problem happened (i.e., causal analysis) and how it can be solved or alleviated (i.e., problem-solving analysis). Previous research analyzing cross-sectional data suggests that causal analysis is directly related to depressive symptoms and precedes problem-solving analysis, but this finding should be supported with experimental methods. We used an expressive writing paradigm to investigate the temporal order of the emotional symptoms of depression, causal analysis, and problem-solving analysis in a non-clinical sample. In two studies, we randomized participants to write about their personal problems or a neutral topic (i.e., control writing). All participants reported on their emotions during writing, and they retrospectively reported on their thoughts after writing. Consistent with our predictions, expressive writing reliably increased self-reported sadness and causal analysis but not problem-solving analysis. Linguistic analyses partially supported our predictions. Relative to control writing, expressive writing contained more words related to causal thoughts and problem-solving. Since causal analysis and sadness increased over the short time frame of the writing intervention, while problem-solving analysis was unaffected, the results support our prediction that causal analysis precedes problem-solving analysis. We discuss the limitations of our studies, as well as EW’s utility for studying the analytical rumination hypothesis.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association (APvA) (2013). (DSM-5) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.

  • Andrews, P. W., & Durisko, Z. (2017). The evolution of depressive phenotypes. In R. J. DeRubeis & D. R. Strunk (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Mood Disorders (pp. 24–36). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Andrews, P. W., & Thomson Jr., J. A. (2009). The bright side of being blue: depression as an adaptation for analyzing complex problems. Psychological Review, 116(3), 620–654.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Angst, J., Paksarian, D., Cui, L., Merikangas, K. R., Hengartner, M. P., Ajdacic-Gross, V., & Rössler, W. (2016). The epidemiology of common mental disorders from age 20 to 50: results from the prospective Zurich cohort Study. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 25(1), 24–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, P. W., Bharwani, A., Lee, K. R., Fox, M., & Thomson Jr, J. A. (2015). Is serotonin an upper or a downer? The evolution of the serotonergic system and its role in depression and the antidepressant response. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 164-188.

  • Aaron T. Beck, (2008) The Evolution of the Cognitive Model of Depression and Its Neurobiological Correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry 165 (8):969-977

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barbic, S. P., Durisko, Z., & Andrews, P. W. (2014). Measuring the bright side of being blue: a new tool for assessing analytical rumination in depression. PloS One, 9(11), e112077.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bartoskova, M., Sevcikova, M., Durisko, Z., Maslej, M., Preiss, M., & Andrews, P. W. (2018). The form and function of depressive rumination. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(3), 277–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beach, S. R., & Amir, N. (2003). Is depression taxonic, dimensional, or both? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(2), 228–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boninger, D. S., Gleicher, F., & Strathman, A. (1994). Counterfactual thinking: from what might have been to what may be. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 297–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A.T. (1987). Cognitive models of depression. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly, 1, 5–37.

  • Copeland, W., Shanahan, L., Costello, E. J., & Angold, A. (2011). Cumulative prevalence of psychiatric disorders by young adulthood: a prospective cohort analysis from the Great Smoky Mountains Study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(3), 252–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cristobal, E., Flavian, C., & Guinaliu, M. (2007). Perceived e-service quality (PeSQ) measurement validation and effects on consumer satisfaction and web site loyalty. Managing service quality: An International Journal, 17(3), 317–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickerson, S. S., Kemeny, M. E., Aziz, N., Kim, K. H., & Fahey, J. L. (2004). Immunological effects of induced shame and guilt. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 124–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durisko, Z., Mulsant, B. H., & Andrews, P. W. (2015). An adaptationist perspective on the etiology of depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 172, 315–323.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durisko, Z., Mulsant, B. H., McKenzie, K., & Andrews, P. W. (2016). Using evolutionary theory to guide mental health research. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 61(3), 159–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J. A., & Weary, G. (1993). Depression and the impression-formation continuum: piecemeal processing despite the availability of category information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 636–645.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J., Weary, G., & Gleicher, F. (1991). Depression and social inferences. Paper presented at the Meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago.

  • Epstude, K., & Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12(2), 168–192.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Frattaroli, J. (2006). Experimental disclosure and its moderators: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 823–865.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • George, D., & Mallery, P. (2003). SPSS for Windows step by step: a simple guide and reference. 11.0 update (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gidron, Y., Peri, T., Connolly, J. F., & Shalev, A. Y. (1996). Written disclosure in posttraumatic stress disorder: is it benefical for the patient? The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 184(8), 505–506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gleicher, F., & Weary, G. (1991). Effect of depression on quantity and quality of social inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 105–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, M. A., & Stone, A. A. (1992). Emotional disclosure about traumas and its relation to health: effects of previous disclosure and trauma severity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(1), 75–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hagen, E. H. (2011). Evolutionary theories of depression: a critical review. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56(12), 716–726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, J. A., Weary, G., & Edwards, J. A. (1999). Certainty-related beliefs and depressive symptomatology: concurrent and longitudinal relationships. Social Cognition, 17(1), 19–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, M. C., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2007). Association of different adverse life events with distinct patterns of depressive symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(10), 1521–1529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kloss, J. D., & Lisman, S. A. (2002). An exposure-based examination of the effects of written emotional disclosure. British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, 31–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovac, S. H., & Range, L. M. (2002). Does writing about suicidal thoughts and feelings reduce them? Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 32(4), 428–440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krpan, K. M., Kross, E., Berman, M. G., Deldin, P. J., Askren, M. K., & Jonides, J. (2013). An everyday activity as a treatment for depression: the benefits of expressive writing for people diagnosed with major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(3), 1148–1151.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2006). Reinterpreting comorbidity: a model-based approach to understanding and classifying psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2, 111–133.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lubin, B. (1965) Adjective Checklists for Measurement of Depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 12 (1):57

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. W., Kim, I., Yoo, J., Park, S., Jeong, B., & Cha, M. (2016). Insights from an expressive writing intervention on Facebook to help alleviate depressive symptoms. Computers in Human Behavior, 62, 613–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P., & Kassam, K. S. (2015). Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 799–823.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem-solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(1), 176–190.

  • Markman, K. D., & Weary, G. (1998). Control motivation, depression, and counterfactual thought. In M. Kofta, G. Weary, & G. Sedek (Eds.), Personal control in action (pp. 363–390). Boston: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, K. D., Gavanski, I., Sherman, S. J., & McMullen, M. N. (1993). The mental simulation of better and worse possible worlds. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29(1), 87–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, K. L., & Weary, G. (1989). Depression and attributional complexity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15(3), 325–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maslej, M. M., Rheaume, A. R., Barbic, S. P., & Andrews, P. W. (unpublished). The Valence Arousal Mood Profile.

  • Matthews, G., Jones, D, M., & Chamberlain, A.G. (1990). Refining the measurement of mood: The UWIST Mood Adjective Checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17–42.

  • Nasco, S. A., & Marsh, K. L. (1996). The effect of upward counterfactuals on subsequent test performance. Paper presented at the Midwestern Psychological Association Conference, Chicago, IL.

  • Pascual-Leone, A., Yeryomenko, N., Morrison, O. P., Arnold, R., & Kramer, U. (2016). Does feeling bad, lead to feeling good? Arousal patterns during expressive writing. Review of General Psychology, 20(3), 336–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W. (1997). Writing about emotional experiences as a therapeutic process. Psychological Science, 8(3), 162–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: toward an understanding of inhibition and disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(3), 274–281.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (1988). Disclosure of traumas and immune function: health implications for psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(2), 239–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J., & Francis, M. (1999). Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count: LIWC, 1999.

  • Roese, N. J. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 805–818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 133–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1997). Counterfactual thinking: the intersection of affect and function. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 1–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., Hur, T., & Pennington, G. L. (1999). Counterfactual thinking and regulatory focus: implications for action versus inaction and sufficiency versus necessity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1109–1120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, D. B. (1987). The calculation of posterior distributions by data augmentation: comment: a noniterative sampling/importance resampling alternative to the data augmentation algorithm for creating a few imputations when fractions of missing information are modest: The SIR algorithm. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 82(398), 543–546.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanna, L. J., & Turley, K. J. (1996). Antecedents to spontaneous counterfactual thinking: effects of expectancy violation and outcome valence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(9), 906–919.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smyth, J. M., Hockemeyer, J. R., & Tulloch, H. (2008). Expressive writing and post-traumatic stress disorder: effects on trauma symptoms, mood states, and cortisol reactivity. British Journal of Health Psychology, 13(1), 85–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tausczik, Y. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). The psychological meaning of words: LIWC and computerized text analysis methods. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29(1), 24–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., & Barrett, H. C. (2003). The second law of thermodynamics is the first law of psychology: Evolutionary developmental psychology and the theory of tandem, coordinated inheritances: Comment on Lickliter and Honeycutt (2003). Psychological Bulletin, 129(6), 858–865.

  • Vanderberg, R. J., & Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 3(1), 4–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Y. P., & Gorenstein, C. (2013). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory-II: a comprehensive review. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 35(4), 416–431.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., & Gannon, K. (1996). Depression, control motivation, and person perception. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behaviour (pp. 146–147). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., Marsh, K. L., Gleicher, F., & Edwards, J. A. (1993). Depression, control motivation, and the processing of information about others. In  K. L. Marsh, F. Gleicher, & G. Weary (Eds.), Control Motivation and Social Cognition (pp. 255–287). New York, NY: Springer.

  • Wells, G. L., & Gavanski, I. (1989). Mental simulation of causality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(2), 161–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The Panas-X. Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule-expanded form. The University of Iowa.

Download references

Funding

This work was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) of Canada. SSHRC had no involvement in the study’s design, in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to submit it for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marta Maslej.

Ethics declarations

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee (The McMaster Research Ethics Board, reference number 2013-203) and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(PDF 112 kb).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Maslej, M., Rheaume, A.R., Schmidt, L.A. et al. Using Expressive Writing to Test an Evolutionary Hypothesis About Depressive Rumination: Sadness Coincides with Causal Analysis of a Personal Problem, Not Problem-solving Analysis. Evolutionary Psychological Science 6, 119–135 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-019-00219-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-019-00219-8

Keywords

Navigation