FormalPara Key Points

Most sleeping pills e.g., benzodiazepines and “Z” benzodiazepine agonists (like eszopiclone) are associated with early death, and they most probably cause serious increases in mortality.

Benzodiazepine agonist sleeping pills and diphenhydramine should not be prescribed for insomnia, except in long-term controlled trials and hospice care.

1 Introduction

The Cancer Prevention Study I of the American Cancer Society collected health questionnaires from over one million participants in the fall and winter of 1959–1960, obtaining 98.4 % ascertainment of survival after 6 years. To examine associations of reported sleep durations and mortality, cross-tabulations were prepared controlling for reported ‘insomnia’ and use of ‘sleeping pills,’ as well as age, gender, and four major morbidities. Like Hammond’s previous pilot analysis [1], the new analyses showed that mortality in both sexes had a U-shaped association with questionnaire-reported sleep duration, with the lowest risk for those reporting 7.0–7.9 h (coded as 7 h in this study) [2]. This association of sleep duration with mortality on 6-year prospective follow-up was little influenced by control for reported insomnia or sleeping pill usage. Reported insomnia was only weakly and equivocally associated with mortality once hours of sleep and sleeping pill usage were controlled, but the presence or absence of reported insomnia or reported short sleep duration had relatively little impact on the association of sleeping pill usage, with about 50 % increased mortality. In this early study, the phraseology of the sleep questions left much to be desired; particularly, lack of specification of which ‘sleeping pills’ were used ‘often.’ Because of the limitations of computer and software capacity at the time of analyzing data from over 1,000,000 participants, the cross-tabulations for potential confounders were extremely rudimentary.

The sleep questions for the Cancer Prevention Study II (CPSII) were clarified in 1982, when new questionnaires were distributed to a new prospective sample of over 1.1 million participants. In particular, the focus was directed to ‘prescription sleeping pills,’ usage was reported in times per month, and prescription sleeping pills were separated from ‘Librium’ and ‘Valium’ as distinct categories. By the late 1990s, with powerful desktop computers becoming available, the coded and compiled data could be analyzed with Cox proportional hazards models simultaneously adjusting for 32 covariates [3, 4]. In the new study, the minimum mortality risk, though still coded as 7 h, indicated that the best survival was with 6.5–7.4 h reported sleep. Some surprises in CPSII were that reported sleep durations <6.5 h were less common and less associated with mortality risk than long reported sleep ≥7.5 h. This might reduce motivations to drug patients to sleep for more than 7.5 h. Reported insomnia predicted slightly (but significantly) improved survival, but greater sleeping pill use retained a dose–response association with decreased survival after adjusting for dozens of covariates [4]. Sleeping pill treatment was associated with much more risk than reported insomnia. Important limitations of the CPSII analyses included lack of definition of the particular prescription sleeping pills utilized and lack of consideration of the extent that participants maintained their prospectively reported use (or lack of use) of sleeping pills throughout the 6-year follow-up. Also, there was no correction for control participants who might have initiated sleeping pill consumption after the prospective baseline assessments. The sparse data concerning subsequent sleeping pill use after the baseline distinguished hypnotics users and controls were not taken into account. Even including 32 questionnaire-derived covariates in the Cox proportional hazards models, there were numerous conceivable confounders that might have been under-adjusted. In addition, adjusting for comorbidities that might have been caused by sleeping pills might have resulted in over-adjustments.

In the decades since these studies were done, although the issue of mortality associated with insomnia is still complex and controversial, dozens of studies have replicated the U-shaped association of reported sleep durations with mortality, and likewise, dozens of studies have replicated the association of hypnotics consumption with mortality. This review will update the list of studies relating mortality risk to hypnotics consumption, critique the limitations of those studies and their strengths, and point out the crucial remaining tasks.

2 Methods

By searching the last 30 years of PubMed publications for key words including both ‘sedative hypnotic’ and ‘mortality,’ 1700 titles were retrieved. The author reviewed each title along with the abstract and text as needed, in order to identify epidemiologic studies of hypnotics or hypnotics and anxiolytics that described follow-up mortality. Along with these publications, the systematic search for studies was supplemented by over 40 years of less formal reading of a much larger and more diverse sleep literature along with retrieval of relevant references listed in that literature. This located 36 epidemiologic studies analyzing whether use of hypnotics (with or without including sedative anxiolytics) predicted subsequent mortality, as listed in Table 1. Most of these studies focused on hypnotics rather than anxiolytics. Some non-English language reports, reports not accessible at the University of California libraries website, and small studies in which the sample size or design was judged too inadequate were omitted. All of those deliberately omitted studies together constituted a small number without deliberate bias.

Table 1 Epidemiologic studies concerning the mortality risk of hypnotics and anxiolytics

3 Key Result: Use of Hypnotics is Certainly Associated with Increased Mortality

All 34 of the studies that listed a mortality risk ratio, hazard ratio, or odds ratio found that the risk of hypnotic and anxiolytic use was >1.0 (in 33 studies) or 1.0 (in one small study). In other words, using hypnotics was followed by earlier deaths (Table 1). Of these, 24 individual studies found this positive association to be statistically significant (p < 0.05). This means that there were 24 studies that reported that hypnotic and sedative–anxiolytic use significantly predicted early mortality. There were no studies reporting any evidence of a protective effect of hypnotics; that is, there were no studies with a risk ratio of <1.0 (Table 1).

4 Limitations

4.1 Power

The power of the study influenced the chances of finding significant association: all 14 reported associations with a sample size >14,000 were significant, but 10 of the 22 studies with sample size <14,000 found a similar trend but not a significant association. Most of the smaller studies failing to find significance in the association were among the earliest 15 published (2005 or earlier).

4.2 Excessive Follow-Up Interval

An over-lengthy interval of prospective mortality follow-up from the baseline determination of hypnotic use was an additional factor that seemed to influence the outcomes. Only 4 of 22 studies that analyzed follow-up of 8 years or less failed to report a significant association, but 6 of 14 studies analyzing follow-up intervals >8 years failed to find significant association. One explanation of this is obvious. Participants who may be utilizing hypnotics at baseline do not necessarily continue to use them for many years. For example, in a study following patients’ mortality for up to 12 years, Jaussent et al. reported that less than one third of those who had taken a hypnotic at baseline or at 2, 4, and 8-year follow-ups were taking a hypnotic at all four assessments (online comment doi:10.1186/1741-7015-11-212 [5]). Moreover, a percentage of patients 69 % as great as those taking a hypnotic at baseline initiated hypnotic usage after baseline during the first 8 years of the 12-year follow-up. Therefore, in the 12-year maximum follow-up, it appears that the number of study patients commencing hypnotic usage after baseline might be almost as great as those using a hypnotic at baseline. Were baseline usage the prospective criterion for dividing hypnotic users from controls, it may be seen that over 12 years, the actual hypnotic consumption of the control group would more and more approximate that of the baseline hypnotic users. If a study separates hypnotic users and controls prospectively at baseline without monitoring hypnotic consumption during follow-up, the longer the study, the more strongly the hypnotic consumption of ‘users’ and ‘controls’ is likely to overlap. Methods of adjustment such as time-dependent proportional hazards models, when feasible at all, have unknown adequacy for adjusting these usage factors. Usage overlap of hypnotic-user cases and their controls tends to weaken the contrast between hypnotic users and controls, leading to underestimation of the true hypnotic-associated risk ratio. The two large American Cancer Society studies both had this limitation, tending towards underestimation of risk [24].

4.3 Confounding Drugs with Different Indications

For perhaps a century, the most popular hypnotics have been drugs that augment the action of ligand-gated anion channels, usually called GABA receptors. GABA acts to open chloride channels that generally lead to intracellular hyperpolarization and consequent inhibition of neuronal firing. Although ethanol and barbiturates react in parallel at other sites on GABA receptors, for decades the predominant hypnotics have been benzodiazepines and benzodiazepine agonists acting on benzodiazepine binding sites on GABA receptors and tending to amplify GABAergic effects [6, 7]. To oversimplify, these drugs tend to shut down brain activity. Histamine and antihistamines may also act on GABA receptors [8], raising a question whether antihistamines with hypnotic properties might also act in part through effects on GABA receptors. Some of the same drugs have hypnotic and anxiolytic properties, as well as muscle-relaxant and anticonvulsant effects. Some of the most important differences among benzodiazepine agonists have to do with the speed of absorption and the metabolic half-life of the drug and active metabolites. For example, a drug such as zaleplon is quickly absorbed but acts for only 1–3 h before it is mostly inactivated. A drug such as zolpidem or triazolam is rapidly absorbed and acts for most of the night, but rapid drug metabolism may produce early awakening or daytime hyper-arousal or anxiety, whereas a drug such as lorazepam may be somewhat more slowly absorbed and long-acting enough to sedate during the day after bedtime administration. Drugs such as diazepam and flurazepam, with their active metabolites, persist in their actions for several days and accumulate (the degree of accumulation depending on metabolic processes that are often slowed in old age). Chlordiazepoxide persists in the body for weeks.

A wise physician may choose a different hypnotic for a patient who has trouble falling asleep compared with a patient who primarily complains of early awakenings. Longer acting drugs may be selected for anxiolytic effects at the risk of accumulating daytime sedation, and drugs intended as anxiolytics are often given in the day rather than at night. Thus, the adverse effects will depend both on the drug and the indication. Because benefits and risks certainly vary according to time of administration and time of day, it may be irrational to combine GABA agonists with widely varying kinetics in the same risk analysis. A short-acting drug given at bedtime may have strong effects on respiratory depression when combined with a narcotic but little effect on next-day driving. A tranquilizer given in the morning will have more effect on driving but less effect on nocturnal respiration.

Only a minority of the studies in Table 1 ascertained the actual drugs being used as sleeping pills. Many patients who take a drug at night for sleep have no idea of its pharmacologic indications, so questionnaire data are often extremely nonspecific as to the drug taken. Some of the studies may have included non-prescription sleeping pills that have historically contained antihistamines such as diphenhydramine, but may have also contained non-steroidal anti-inflammatory or scopolamine-like drugs. Most of the epidemiologic studies of hypnotics to date have been confounding short-acting hypnotics and long-acting anxiolytics, along with antihistamines, nonsteroidal anti-inflammatories, antidepressants, and neuroleptics, usually in unknown proportions. Such confounding no doubt impacts the reported risks in ways that are quite unknowable. Although a patient may report any variety of drugs as sleeping pills, many of the drugs have no currently recognized medical indication for sleep at all, so that whether a drug with an other-than-hypnotic indication does or does not influence mortality risk, its inclusion obscures precise focus on the risks of true hypnotics.

In large electronic records studies [9, 10], it has appeared that all benzodiazepine agonists examined (including ‘Z’ drugs) are associated with mortality risks. Diphenhydramine and possibly other similar antihistamines have also been implicated in mortality risks [10, 11]. Clear epidemiologic studies of mortality risks seem unavailable for trazodone, doxepin (especially low-dose doxepin), and melatonin when used as hypnotics.

4.4 Unknown Dosages Consumed

Very few of the studies listed in Table 1 used pharmacy records of what quantities of hypnotics were actually dispensed, and few studies had data on the numbers of doses prescribed over the interval of follow-up. Needless to mention, dispensed hypnotics often sit in the medicine chest with most doses unused, but other doses may be shared with relatives or may even be resold more widely. Having at best only rough estimates of the quantities of drugs that patients in the hypnotics-taking groups consumed, even the best epidemiologic studies of hypnotic risks are only poorly able to examine dose response. Of course, even when the defined daily dosage (DDD) of a drug a study participant has consumed can be estimated, drug effects are known to vary widely by age, gender, and body mass of the patient, metabolic considerations (especially CYP3A enzyme inhibitor effects), and drug combinations, so the implications of dosages are still quite controversial.

4.5 No Formal Meta-Analysis

The reader who understands what meta-analyses can do will immediately understand that with 33 out of 34 epidemiologic studies pointing in the same direction, and with high levels of statistical reliability in many of the single studies, meta-analysis is not necessary to be assured of the statistical significance of the hypnotics–mortality association. At the same time, the studies listed were exceptionally heterogeneous in the nature of the drugs studied; knowledge of the actual compounds studied and the quantities actually consumed by research participants; the numbers of participants; characteristics of participants including age, gender, health status, and other-risk exposures; the durations of follow-up; and the statistical details, especially adjustments of potential confounders. There are so many important sources of heterogeneity between studies that the author sees no satisfactory method of selecting homogeneous subgroups of studies for meta-analyses. Meta-analysis would only produce additional risks of confounding due to study selection. Several of the studies provided such statistically firm results in themselves that combination with other studies would be superfluous. Three such studies will be discussed at some length, each of which used electronic prescription records to unfold the prescriptions patients received for specific drugs during follow-up and to some extent before the baseline as well.

Weich et al. examined a representative population sample from the UK General Practice Research Database [9]. Hypnotic users had received over a 4-year interval at least two prescriptions for a benzodiazepine (both hypnotics and anxiolytics), Z hypnotic, or other hypnotic, and had not received such drugs prior to study initiation. Considering patients who had received a study drug in the first year of recruitment only versus matched hypnotic-free controls, fully adjusted hazard ratios for hypnotic usage predicting mortality ranged from 1.0 (no exposure, the reference) to 2.55 (95 % CI 2.42–2.69) for 1–30 DDDs, 3.78 (95 % CI 3.54–4.04) for 31–60 DDDs, 4.19 (95 % CI 3.84–4.58) for 61–90 DDDs, and 4.51 (95 % CI 4.22–4.82) for >91 DDDs. All of these mortality associations for hypnotics exposures were highly significant (p < 0.001). The hazard ratios were highest among patients receiving benzodiazepines only and somewhat lower among patients receiving Z drugs only or other hypnotics only, but all demonstrated significant dose response. These hazard ratios were fully adjusted for sex, age, sleep disorders, anxiety or other psychiatric disorders, and many medical morbidities such as heart disease, diabetes mellitus, and cancer. Comorbidities were considered prospectively, and concurrent comorbidity was considered separately. Either prospective or concurrent covariate adjustment reduced the hazard ratios rather little. The modest impact of either prospective or concurrent extensive full adjustment for comorbidities and other confounders suggested that there was little confounding by indication (that is, confounding because study drugs were selectively given to patients because they were already perceived as ill.) The investigators suggested “that much of the excess mortality risk arises early in the period of drug use but remains statistically significant even after discontinuing the drugs.” Nevertheless, the investigators noted that residual confounding could not be excluded; for example, due to confounders that could not be adequately measured.

Kripke et al. examined mortality among patients of a large rural US managed-care organization using electronic medical records of medical diagnoses, a cancer registry, and prescriptions [10]. Patients who received a hypnotic were matched on age, gender, smoking, and various prior and concurrent morbidities with controls not receiving hypnotics and followed for an average of 2.5 years. The association of hypnotics with increased mortality was highly significant (p < 0.001). Zolpidem and temazepam were the hypnotics most commonly prescribed. Only indicated hypnotics prescribed at bedtime were included in this analysis. As in the Weich et al. study [9], a dose–response relationship with hypnotic prescribing was noted, but much of the mortality hazard was associated with low doses and/or short-term prescribing. Considering the many design differences between these two studies, the hazard ratios were surprisingly similar. As in the UK study, extensive and varied efforts to adjust for confounding by intention to treat and to isolate various comorbidities suggested that confounding had only a small impact on a study done in this way. However, the authors admitted that some small element of confounding was likely.

Obiora et al. examined pneumonia mortality in 4964 cases and 29,697 controls also selected from the UK primary care patient database [13]. Using the Charlson’s comorbidity index to adjust for comorbidities, as compared with controls, fully adjusted long-term mortality hazard ratios were 1.42 (95 % CI 1.21–1.67) for current benzodiazepine use, 1.49 (95 % CI 1.19–1.85) for recent benzodiazepine use, and 1.24 (95 % CI 1.09–1.41) for past benzodiazepine use. There were few zopiclone data, but for current use, the adjusted hazard ratio was likewise significant at 2.17 (95 % CI 1.17–4.06).

The relatively high and highly significant hazard ratios observed in these studies indicate the value of using electronic databases to identify hypnotic prescriptions, comorbidities, and associated mortality. These studies demonstrate an association of hypnotic use with subsequent mortality risk that needs no meta-analysis for confirmation. Although these studies made extensive attempts to avoid confounding and to counteract it, using highly developed databases, none was able to claim total freedom from risks of confounding factors. As a consequence, the causality of hypnotics causing excess mortality remained somewhat open to challenge.

It is worth observing that these three studies yielded among the highest hazard ratios of the 34 studies listed in Table 1. Probably-important factors in the relatively high hazard estimates were exclusion or limitation of patients using hypnotics before the baseline observations; the relatively brief follow-ups enabling ongoing monitoring of hypnotic prescriptions among both those classed as hypnotic users and matched controls, thus minimizing intermingling of the groups; the availability of dose–response data; and the quality of information about comorbidities as well as smoking and BMI.

4.6 Confounding by Indication

To show a large and statistically very significant association between hypnotics and the subsequent risk of early mortality is not absolute proof that the hypnotics cause that mortality.

A recent publication presented evidence that hypnotic prescribing is increased for a few months before death for patients possibly manifesting terminal illnesses [14]. This was interpreted as confounding by indication (i.e., the terminal illness caused the hypnotic usage). Such terminal prescribing may be appropriate, since hypnotics might be useful hospice agents in situations where prolonging a patient’s life is hopeless and the amnesic and anxiolytic effects of hypnotics seem desirable. However, the same data set showed that 2 years before death, usage was already elevated among patients who would die, but roughly parallel increases in hypnotic use were observed among patients who did or did not die in the first year of the 2-year observations [14]. This is consistent with hypnotics causing terminal conditions, but the increase in hypnotic usage in the year before death also seemed consistent with hypnotics rapidly causing terminal conditions. Such data by themselves do not at all argue that the association of hypnotics with excess mortality can be entirely attributed to confounding by indication, and unfortunately, the study did not control for comorbidities.

If a physician prescribes a hypnotic for insomnia or for depression with insomnia, commentators worry that it is the insomnia and not the hypnotic that causes the associated mortality. However, insomnia and depression are probably not important sources of confounding, since several studies found that adjusting for these comorbidities made little change in the hazard ratios for hypnotics [4, 1517].

A physician may prescribe a hypnotic because of a suspicion that the patient needs to sleep better, even when no complaint of insomnia is present or no diagnosis of a sleep disorder happens to be recorded. A physician might believe (without it being an accepted indication) that hypnotics are good for hypertension or heart disease or obesity or ulcers, etc., and thus the hypnotic prescription could be a sign of diverse dire disorders. If these other disorders are accurately diagnosed, then these confounding sources of mortality risk can be managed by statistical adjustments for comorbidities (see Figs. 1, 2, 3). If no prospective medical diagnosis is made of the disorder being treated by the hypnotic (for which statistical adjustment would be possible), but the physician felt through empathy or the bedside manner or something more mysterious that there was a need for a hypnotic, then the confounding indications probably cannot be adequately managed statistically.

Fig. 1
figure 1

A causal model in which hypnotic drugs and various potential confounders have completely independent effects on mortality. A Cox proportional hazards model should easily separate these independent covariates of mortality

Fig. 2
figure 2

A causal model in which confounders influence hypnotic use and independently influence mortality, but hypnotic drugs are intended to treat the primary confounders and have no direct influence on mortality. In this case, there will appear to be an association between hypnotic drug use and mortality that is completely confounded by intention and wholly attributable to the correlations of both hypnotic use and mortality with the confounders. However, the proportional hazards model could reject hypnotic drug use as a hazard, since adjusting for the confounders would better explain the variance, unless certain confounders were inadequately or imprecisely specified

Fig. 3
figure 3

A causal model in which confounding factors influence mortality directly and also indirectly through the pathway mediated through confounder-modulation of hypnotic drug use. Although the intention in prescribing a hypnotic may be to treat the confounding condition rather than to treat insomnia, to the extent that the causal pathway is mediated by hypnotic drug prescribing, the hypnotic drugs have a preventable causal role. This model also illustrates that hypnotic prescribing initiated independent of confounders (e.g., in treatment of ‘primary’ insomnia) may also influence drug use

There is evidence for much mysterious prescribing of hypnotics, of which the US National Ambulatory Medical Care Survey provided useful weighted estimates from a national sample [18]. In 2009, there were an estimated 5.2 million office visits in which insomnia was a stated reason for the visit and a total 10,224,000 visits for any sleep disturbance (including disturbances for which prescribing a hypnotic would not be appropriate, such as most cases of sleep apnea). A sleep-related diagnosis (not just insomnia; e.g., includes sleep apnea) was listed in 15.2 million visits, but there were 23.3 million visits upon which a prescription was given for a hypnotic medication FDA-indicated for insomnia. Expanding the enumeration to include such medications as alprazolam, clonazepam, diazepam, flunitrazapam, lorazepam, etc. that are commonly prescribed for sleep, a total of about 60 million visits with prescriptions for drugs often (not always) used as sleeping pills was arrived at. These estimates are murky, but the National Survey data seem to show that as much as 40–90 % of the times physicians prescribe hypnotics and related anxiolytics, they record no indication of insomnia or other recognized medical indication for the prescription. There is very little knowledge of why this might occur. The National Ambulatory Medical Care Survey is accessible contemporary documentation of how hypnotic drug prescriptions are written, but it is not alone. In the sparse literature available about why physicians prescribe hypnotics, there has been much evidence that there is often no articulated belief that the hypnotic prescribed is indicated for insomnia or a related sleep disturbance, and often the patient denies insomnia and does not know why the prescription was given [19, 20]. Current electronic medical audit systems are increasingly encouraging physicians who wish to prescribe hypnotics to check off a diagnosis of insomnia, but no evidence has been located that checking off this diagnosis on a computer screen would reliably indicate that sleep symptoms had actually been discussed with the patient, much less that current criteria for insomnia disorder had been met. It is hard to estimate how often a hypnotic prescription was offered for a confounding indication and how often there was no medical indication at all.

There may be no authoritative documentation, but young hospital physicians are sometimes instructed to prescribe hypnotics so that they will not be awakened at night for requests for such as-needed prescriptions. In such cases, the hypnotic prescriptions are to allow the physicians to sleep, not the patients. Allow me to document that this was my training at prestigious hospitals. Physicians sometimes say that hospital night nurses insist that their patients have hypnotic prescriptions in hopes that they (the nurses) will have a less difficult night. Perhaps these prescriptions are so that they (the night nurses) can get some sleep. If these are the rationales for such prescriptions, this would not produce confounding of medical indications for hypnotics, but it is hard to exclude confounding risks related to caregiver fatigue. Such habits of automatic prescribing might carry over from hospitals into the outpatient arena.

There may be no published documentation, but physicians have repeatedly told me that they prescribe hypnotics in fear that unsatisfied patients will depart for new physicians. Physicians are fond of their patients, but it is difficult to exclude any financial motivation to retain patients. Unstated is the tendency for patients prescribed addicting drugs to return for refills to avoid withdrawal symptoms. Years ago, one of the country’s most distinguished sleep physicians said he had surveyed a group of outstanding clinicians, who explained that in prescribing hypnotics, they selected patients whom they knew well and trusted. The described gift-giving also implied the use of hypnotics to maintain doctor–patient relationships rather than to treat sleep symptoms. It seems currently unknown if such attitudes are correlated with mortality risks, thus constituting a source of confounding by indication.

To summarize, almost all of the studies listed in Table 1 made some attempts to adjust for confounding by indication by adjusting for comorbidities and other risk factors that might have confounded the associations of hypnotics with mortality risk. The three studies emphasized in Sect. 4.5 planned very extensive efforts to adjust for comorbidities, and two used matching of cases to controls, but observed that final models with full adjustment of many covariates reduced the estimated risk ratios rather little. It is true that electronic-record comorbidity data may have had limited accuracy and precision, and there are too many small or uncommon sources of comorbidity risks to adjust for all of them, but if confounding by medical indication entirely explained the associations, greater success with the adjustments utilized might have been expected. Considering that many hypnotic prescriptions are not explained by documented medical indications, in such cases, the operative motivating indications for prescription are unknown and possibly pseudo-randomly inspired. It seems unlikely that such undocumented indications are strong mortality risk predictors, yet it is likewise impossible to exclude confounding by such mysterious unknown indications.

4.7 Lack of Data for Cause and Time of Death

An important limitation in almost all of the epidemiology about hypnotics risks is lack of data on the causes of death and times of death associated with hypnotics usage. Although such information is not essential to demonstrating the statistical significance of the hypnotics–mortality association, more information on causes of death would be helpful in understanding possible causal mechanisms by which hypnotics may increase mortality risks. A distinct pattern of causes of death would add to the evidence for causality. Moreover, it might be found that deaths of hypnotics users occur out-of-proportion at night, as some old data suggested [21]. If hypnotics use is associated with death at night, that would support the hypothesis that hypnotics may cause covert nocturnal compromise of respiration.

4.8 Over-Adjustment of Confounders

Use of hypnotics is associated with a wide variety of comorbidities [9, 10]. Generally, it has been assumed that these comorbidities are confounding indications for hypnotics, as was discussed in Sect. 4.6. What is often not recognized is that hypnotics cause comorbidities such as insomnia, depression, infection, gastric regurgitation, and even perhaps cancer, as proven by industry-sponsored randomized controlled clinical trials [2226]. Also, controlled trials prove that many hypnotics impair next-day driving performance. Moreover, hypnotics are epidemiologically associated with an increased rate of driver-at-fault vehicle crashes as well as falls and serious infections [13, 2736]. Other studies show that hypnotic use is associated with new cancer incidence [3739]. Such studies fall somewhat short of causal proof that hypnotics cause deaths from injuries, infections, suicide, and cancer, but for purposes of epidemiologic studies, it should be assumed that hypnotic prescribing may produce reverse causation of several forms of comorbidity, as suggested in Fig. 4.

Fig. 4
figure 4

A causal model in which bidirectional interactions occur between hypnotic drug prescribing and confounders. Examples of such bidirectional causal interactions would be those between hypnotics and depression or hypnotics and infections. In this model, there is a pathway of hypnotic drug prescribing mediating comorbid illness that may then have effects on mortality. In addition, as in Fig. 3, confounders may mediate mortality effects through influences upon hypnotic usage. It is doubtful that the causal pathways could be distinguished without some approach resembling path analysis with structural equation modeling

That hypnotics can cause comorbidities raises problems in analysis of the association of hypnotics and mortality. To adjust for confounding of indications, it may seem desirable to adjust for comorbidities such as insomnia, depression, infection, and cancer. However, to the extent that such comorbidities are caused by the hypnotics, this would be over-adjustment. Over-adjustment may in effect assign estimated variance in mortality to comorbidities that are primarily caused by the hypnotics, leading to underestimation of mortality association that should be attributed to the hypnotics. Studies such as that of Jaussent et al., in which the association of hypnotic use and mortality lost significance once adjusted for depression, etc., might suffer from such over-adjustment and might obscure the true risk associated with hypnotics [5]. If the patients in the study have received hypnotics before the baseline assessment, this prior usage before the time of baseline assessment may also confound true hypnotic effects with the comorbidities that have already resulted from hypnotics.

Over-adjustment of comorbidities, excessively long prospective follow-ups with overlap between hypnotic-user groups and control groups in terms of hypnotics consumption during follow-up, inadequate specification of when the drugs studied are true hypnotics, and inadequate specification of drug dosage consumed may all lead to underestimation of the true association of hypnotics with mortality risks.

4.9 Limitations due to Genetic Effects and New Research Possibilities

Genetic polymorphisms probably influence hypnotics consumption, either through causing insomnia and circadian rhythm sleep disorders or through other influences on hypnotics consumption. Perhaps hypnotics consumption is modified by genetic metabolic alterations analogous to effects of alcohol dehydrogenase polymorphisms on alcoholism [40, 41]. There is a heritable influence upon insomnia, but this has not been distinguished from possible heritable effects upon hypnotic usage [42, 43]. As yet, polymorphisms influencing hypnotics consumption independent of insomnia and depression have not been recognized.

If polymorphisms influence hypnotic consumption and independently influence mortality (e.g., see Fig. 2 model), then genetic influences could confound estimates of the association of hypnotic usage and mortality.

From a more scientifically hopeful point of view, genetic factors have potential to clarify the causal component of hypnotic effects on mortality. Consider the more complete (and most probable) causal model that is illustrated in Fig. 5. Note that Fig. 5 does not necessarily imply that the same polymorphisms influence hypnotic drug use and directly influence mortality. There may be genetic polymorphisms that only influence mortality mediated through influences on hypnotics consumption. Genetic factors are unique from other potential confounders in that their causality pathways are one-way. Neither hypnotic usage nor other confounders alter genetic polymorphisms. Rather, if a sample has racially homogeneous ancestry and at least some of the population are heterozygous for a genetic polymorphism, the presence of that polymorphism in offspring may be regarded as randomly determined. This is the basis of Mendelian randomization analyses that consider genetic effects on an instrumental variable (e.g., hypnotics consumption) and an outcome variable (e.g., mortality) in order to estimate the component size of the causal pathway leading from genotype to hypnotics consumption to modified mortality [4447]. Mendelian randomization eliminates the interference of most confounders in estimating causality, though there remain various complex issues that must be considered. It now seems feasible to perform such causality studies, but one does not know when results might be forthcoming.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Revises the model in Fig. 4 to emphasize that genetic factors may influence confounders such as comorbid illnesses or cigarette smoking and cause mortality indirectly through these pathways, as well as through what may be conceived as direct effects on mortality. However, in general, neither hypnotic drugs nor confounders such as comorbid illnesses or life experiences can influence our genetic inheritance

5 Discussion

5.1 The Causality of Hypnotics Effects on Mortality

It has been amply demonstrated that hypnotics consumption predicts increased future mortality. Although the 34 studies in Table 1 are heterogeneous in the limitations that may lead to overestimation and underestimation of true causes of risk (which may explain the high variability of observed risk ratios), there was a persuasive consensus that hypnotics risk ratios were positive. Given the many limitations of epidemiologic studies demonstrating associations of hypnotics consumption with mortality risks, with several possibilities for overestimation and underestimation of the risks attributable to hypnotics, it can be argued that no epidemiologic method relying on clinical observation alone can provide accurate scientific assessment of the mortality caused by hypnotics. Since some of the best-adjusted studies yielded among the highest risk ratios [9, 10, 15], it appears that limitations tending towards underestimation of hypnotics risk often over-balanced the limitations tending towards overestimation. It seems unlikely (but not implausible) that so much mortality risk could be entirely explained by unknown confounders.

Of course, that hypnotics cause some small percentage of deaths is not really open to question. Recently, several US jurisdictions have concluded that injecting hypnotic drugs are among the preferred methods of executing condemned prisoners [48]. Drugs that have been used as hypnotics such as barbiturates and midazolam have been employed. Barbiturates have for decades been a preferred method of euthanizing experimental animals [48, 49]. For these applications, the mechanisms by which hypnotics hyperpolarize and inhibit respiratory neurons leading to death are well understood at the molecular, cellular, physiologic and organismic levels. Also reported are thousands of overdose deaths per year attributable to hypnotics, with a larger number of cases in which hypnotics as well as opiates and other sedatives are lethal in combination [5052]. It has been suggested that currently, tens of thousands of yearly self-injury deaths are related to hypnotics and sedative use [53].

In addition to recognized overdoses, it is likely that many hypnotics-induced deaths may be unrecognized and under-reported. Because most patients who die unexpectedly at night are not autopsied, or because adequate assays of all hypnotics present at death are not available, or too much time for post-mortem drug metabolism and degradation has occurred, the overdose may not be recognized. Normal approved hypnotic dosages might even be lethal under specific circumstances. Although the lethal dosage of benzodiazepine agonists is usually as much as hundreds of hypnotic doses, it drops by an extraordinary degree after moderate ingestions of alcohol [54, 55]. Combinations of narcotics and hypnotics, especially with the addition of alcohol and other sedatives, may slow breathing [56]. There are no established safe dose ranges for combinations of hypnotics, sedatives, and narcotics. If an obese, elderly patient receiving accepted doses of hypnotics and opiates dies unexpectedly at night, how could a physician document that the drugs were responsible? Even when a patient dies under observation with respiratory and cardiac monitoring, it may be difficult to demonstrate that an unintended drug-induced suppression of respiration led to the terminal event. Using data given in the supplement to reference [10], it can be estimated that only one death per 5000 hypnotic doses consumed would suffice to cause the hundreds of thousands of US deaths associated with hypnotics. Is it not plausible that such a tiny rate of lethal outcomes could occur? Even when a physician signing a death certificate suspects that hypnotics may have played a role, the physician usually lacks proof and may be reluctant to attribute the death to a drug that the physician may have prescribed. It is easier to attribute the death to some natural comorbid process for which nobody is responsible, leading to under-reporting of hypnotics-caused deaths.

5.2 Bradford Hill Criteria of Causation are Fulfilled

Review of the nine Bradford Hill criteria of causation support the hypothesis of hypnotics causing excess mortality. (1) The strength of association is strong, especially in recent studies that utilize electronic records data identifying hypnotic prescriptions. (2) Consistency is the strongest evidence, with 33 of 34 studies showing a positive association of hypnotic consumption and mortality. (3) Several kinds of specificity of hypnotics–mortality associations have been identified: the excess of deaths at night among hypnotics users [21], the excesses of infection [13], COPD [57], and suicide deaths [50], and the excess of specific cancers such as lung and esophagus [38]. (4) The temporality of hypnotic usage preceding death is consistent with causation. (5) The significant dose–response data are consistent with causation [9]. (6) Causality is plausible, because the mechanisms of benzodiazepine-agonist suppression of respiration are well understood, hypnotic effects causing depression which causes suicide are understood, immune effects of these drugs make infection plausible, and clastogenicity and immune interference make cancer plausible. (7) The analogies between use of hypnotics for executions and euthanasia, the Medical Examiner determinations of overdose deaths, and the larger picture of hypnotic-associated mortality are highly coherent. (8) Experimental studies such as laboratory experiments demonstrating hypnotics causing overdose deaths in animals, and laboratory experiments causing infections and cancers in animals, and so forth, provide strong evidence. (9) The strong analogies of hypnotics-associated mortality with cigarette-associated mortality, alcoholism-associated mortality, and other addictive drug lethalities support the lethality of hypnotics. Despite how completely the evidence fulfills the Bradford Hill criteria for epidemiologic proof of causation, the impossibility of ruling out all confounders of the hypnotics–mortality association has led to popular demand for stronger evidence that can probably only be derived from other approaches.

5.3 What is Needed for Augmented Scientific Conclusiveness?

To obtain scientifically conclusive assessments of mortality risks posed by hypnotics, there are two scientific strategies available. The first is the Mendelian randomization strategy discussed in Sect. 4.9 that may become increasingly feasible as more and more electronic medical records contain both hypnotic drug administration data and patient genotypes. However, a Mendelian randomization study of hypnotic risks would require identification of genetic polymorphisms that (1) predispose towards increased or decreased hypnotics consumption, and (2) do not influence mortality through pathways independent of their effects on hypnotics consumption. It is not yet known whether such polymorphisms can be identified. The alternative is the classic gold standard of causal proof: randomized controlled clinical trials.

Unfortunately, there are no randomized placebo-controlled clinical trials of hypnotics in mortality-susceptible samples large enough and long enough to determine with reasonable statistical power whether hypnotics cause excess mortality. Industry trials have avoided the most mortality-vulnerable part of the population who receive hypnotics. Several studies have suggested that hypnotics are associated with a magnitude of mortality risks comparable to that of cigarettes [3, 10, 15]. Nevertheless, recognize that no cigarette manufacturers have attempted large clinical trials hoping to demonstrate that their products are without serious mortality risk. Likewise, no hypnotics manufacturer has attempted a large clinical trial intended to demonstrate that their hypnotic is without serious mortality risk. The reasons would be anybody’s guess, assuming the manufacturers believe that their products would be proven safe. A prestigious medical journal published one light-hearted commentary suggesting that the problem might be similar to our lack of controlled studies concerning the consequences of jumping out of airplanes without parachutes [58]. This may imply an ethical difficulty in performing studies of lethal risks, but it is ethical (with informed consent) to randomize patients to hypnotic drugs or placebo under conditions when the participants would be prescribed the drug on clinical grounds outside of research. Such large drug studies are frequently done in cardiology among vulnerable patients. In contrast, it is unethical not to randomize and to determine what the risk is that millions currently undergo. Such studies have now become ethically obligatory. Apart from the classic double-blind randomized drug administration trial, there are interesting new ethical approaches to randomly modifying hypnotic consumption among large groups of patients [59], though such an approach would seem difficult to implement double-blind. Unfortunately, clinical medicine may wait a long time before any investigator finds a sponsor for such a study unless the regulatory authorities take their responsibility to protect the public seriously enough to insist that manufacturers perform adequate mortality-risk studies.

5.4 What is Recommended Awaiting Immaculate Evidence of How Much of Hypnotic Mortality Risks are Causal?

There is conclusive evidence that use of hypnotics is associated with substantially increased mortality in the population. There is conclusive proof that hypnotic drugs are lethal in overdoses and in animals. What should be done while no scientifically conclusive estimate of the population causal mortality risk of hypnotics is available? Remember, there is no indication whatsoever that hypnotics used for sleep ever save lives. In medicine, a physician must use the best data available to make clinical decisions, even when complete evidence is lacking, and even when one might wish there were a better evidence base.

The best example of how medical experts should act is the belated medical leadership on cigarettes. Several studies have suggested that the population mortality risk associated with hypnotics is quite comparable or greater than that of cigarettes, whereas other studies have suggested that the hypnotic-associated risk could be somewhat smaller [3, 10, 15]. The American Cancer Society’s Cancer Prevention Study II (CPSII, previously discussed) is an important example, because until quite recently, US public health authorities and United Nations authorities considered CPSII as providing the most authoritative estimate of population cigarette mortality causality. As discussed, in one analysis of CPSII data, the risk of hypnotics was rather similar to that of cigarettes when considered simultaneously in the very same Cox proportional hazards models using exactly the same data sampling methods and covariate adjustments [3]. There are no randomized controlled trials adequately proving that cigarettes cause deaths, but the epidemiologic data of CPSII and many supporting studies have been considered sufficient evidence for physicians to completely avoid recommending cigarettes. Considering that hypnotics are favorite drugs for euthanizing animals and executing prisoners, cigarettes being unsatisfactory for these purposes, why not conclude that hypnotics likely carry mortality risks close to the same order of magnitude as cigarettes? Why not rely on the same CPSII evidence to take similar action to limit hypnotic consumption?

Official US FDA-approved prescribing information lists all kinds of risks of hypnotics for which the evidence is laughably weak compared with the evidence regarding mortality. The mortality risks of hypnotics should be listed. There should be warnings on the bottles. Are 24 significant positive studies not enough? Until there is better evidence of safety, the reasonable medical procedure is not to wait forever but to act. The reasonable procedure would be to restrict hypnotic drugs to controlled trials and to compassionate hospice use—unless and until such hypnotics might be proven safe in Mendelian randomization studies or in randomized placebo-controlled trials.

For general clinical use as hypnotics, the risks outweigh benefits for benzodiazepine agonists and diphenhydramine used as hypnotics. The risk of killing patients outweighs any benefits. These drugs should not be prescribed as hypnotics except in further clinical trials and hospice situations. In general, it appears that cognitive behavioral treatment of insomnia [60] and bright light treatment for circadian rhythm disorders linked to insomnia [61] are the safest and probably also the most effective treatments for insomnia. Cognitive behavioral treatment of insomnia or bright light, often used in combination, should be the first choice. For trazodone, doxepin, and melatonin when used for sleep, there are no adequate data to estimate whether long-term risks outweigh benefits. European authorities concluded that the benefits/risks ratio of melatonin is probably better than that of ramelteon, a melatonin agonist [19], so there is no reason to use ramelteon. If a decision is made that some medication for insomnia must be risked, it appears that trazodone in minimal doses, doxepin (in low dosage, e.g., 6 mg), or melatonin would be the least-risky options currently available, and when used, these drugs should generally be combined with cognitive behavioral treatment or bright light treatment.

6 Conclusion

A remarkable 33 of 34 epidemiologic studies have found an elevated mortality risk among hypnotics users. That some component of the associated deaths is causal is not open to question because of Medical Examiner-verified overdose deaths. Hypnotics may also cause unrecognized respiratory deaths due to combinations of hypnotics with narcotics, other sedatives, alcohol, obesity, age-related sleep apnea, and other comorbidities. Hypnotics further cause accidents, infections, depression, and possibly cancers, which may lead to death. The evidence of mortality risk among the best-studied hypnotics is strong enough to restrict these drugs to hospice care, though the manufacturers should be permitted to conduct randomized placebo-controlled trials with patients with high or moderate mortality risk designed to prove (if possible) that the hypnotics are safe. Otherwise, since manufacturers have so far declined their option to do large mortality–safety trials, and it is difficult to exclude any confounders influencing epidemiologic association of hypnotic usage with deathFootnote 1, further absolute proof of causality may await Mendelian randomization studies.